Germann, C. B.. (2019). 5-Methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine: An Ego-Dissolving Endogenous Neurochemical Catalyst of Creativity. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 61(4), 170–216.

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s41470-019-00063-y
DOI URL
directSciHub download


PDF: http://psychedelic-lab.com/wp-content/uploads/10.1007_s41470-019-00063-y.pdf
HTML5: http://psychedelic-lab.com/wp-content/uploads/10.1007_s41470-019-00063-y.html
Plain text: http://psychedelic-lab.com/wp-content/uploads/10.1007_s41470-019-00063-y.txt

Slide
Psychedelic Lab


10.1007_s41470-019-00063-y
TOC


Abstract
5-Methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine (acronymized as 5-MeO-DMT) is sui generis among the numerous naturally-occurring psychoactive substances due to its unparalleled ego-dissolving effects which can culminate in a state of nondual consciousness that is phenomenologically similar to transformative peak experiences described in various ancient contemplative traditions (e.g., Advaita Vedānta, Mahāyāna Buddhism, inter alia). The enigmatic molecule is endogenous to the human brain and has profound psychological effects which are hitherto only very poorly understood due to the absence of scientifically controlled human experimental trials. Its exact neuronal receptor binding profile is a matter of ongoing research; however, empirical evidence indicates that its remarkable psychoactivity is partially mediated via agonism of the 5-HT1A/2A (serotonin) receptor subtypes. Anthropological/ethnopharmacological evidence indicates that various cultures utilized 5-MeO-DMT containing plants for medicinal, psychological, and spiritual purposes for millennia. We propose that this naturally occurring serotonergic compound could be fruitfully utilized as a neurochemical research tool with the potential to significantly advance our understanding of the psychological and neuronal processes which underpin cognition and creativity (e.g., downregulation of the default-mode network, increased global functional connectivity, neuroplasticity, σ1 receptor interactions, etc.). An eclectic interdisciplinary perspective is adopted, and we present converging evidence from a plurality of sources in support of our conjecture. Specifically, we argue that 5-MeO-DMT has significant neuropsychopharmacological potential due to its incommensurable capacity to completely disintegrate self-referential cognitive/neuronal processes (viz., ego death). The importance of unbiased systematic scientific research on naturally occurring endogenous psychoactive compounds is discussed from a Jamesian radical empiricism perspective and potential scenarios of abuse are addressed, particularly in the context of neuroethics, cybernetic manipulation, and military research on torture.


Keywords: 5-MeO-DMT, 5-HT2A agonism, Creativity, Nonduality, Anthropocene, Cognitive liberty, Neuroethics.


Introduction
The following prefatory quotation is adapted from Abraham Maslow’s seminal book “Towards a psychology of being” and it provides an apt primer and semantic grounding for the subsequent disquisition.

“An essential aspect of SA [Self-Actualized] creativeness was a special kind of perceptiveness that is exemplified by the child in the fable who saw that the king had no clothes on – this too contradicts the notion of creativity as products. Such people can see the fresh, the raw, the concrete, the ideographic, as well as the generic, the abstract, the rubricized, the categorized and the classified. Consequently, they live far more in the real world of nature than in the verbalized world of concepts, abstractions, expectations, beliefs and stereotypes that most people confuse with the real world. This is well expressed in [Carl] Rogers’ phrase ‘openness to experience’” (Maslow, 1968, p. 145, content in brackets added).


Humanity is currently de facto confronted with an unprecedented existential crisis which could be described as an “anthropogenic planetary emergency”. One major acute threat to the survival of the species comes from the military and the constant threat of nuclear annihilation, another from the destruction of the global ecosystem and the significant and extremely worrisome anthropogenic (man-made) reduction of biodiversity which will soon cause a global systemic collapse (Steffen et al., 2018). The term “biological annihilation” has been proposed to describe this ongoing scenario (Ceballos, Ehrlich, & Dirzo, 2017).
In 1798, Thomas Malthus predicted exponential population growth in his “Essay on the Principle of Population” which influenced the ratification of the Census Act 1800 (41 Geo. III c.15) in Great Britain. Malthus foresaw numerous contemporary challenges and the topic of overpopulation has since then been centrally discussed by numerous influential thinkers such as Sir Charles Galton Darwin, Sir Julian Huxley, and Nobel laureate Lord Bertrand Russell, inter alia. In 1952, Russel wrote the following in his seminal book entitled “The Impact of Science on Society”:

“The danger of a world shortage of food may be averted for a time by improvements in the technique of agriculture. But, if population continues to increase at the present rate, such improvements cannot long suffice. There will then be two groups, one poor with an increasing population, the other rich with a stationary population. Such a situation can hardly fail to lead to world war. If there is not to be an endless succession of wars, population will have to become stationary throughout the world, and this will probably have to be done, in many countries, as a result of governmental measures. This will require an extension of scientific technique into very intimate matters. There are, however, two other possibilities. War may become so destructive that, at any rate for a time, there is no danger of overpopulation; or the scientific nations may be defeated and anarchy may destroy scientific technique.” (Russell, 1952, p. 27)

In 1960, Heinz von Förster et al. published a paper in SCIENCE MAGAZINE which introduced the “Doomsday Equation”. Based on mathematical extrapolation of an elongated J-curve, this equation predicts that population growth would become infinite at a specific (finite) point in time, that is, by Friday the 13th of November, A.D. 2026 (von Förster, Mora, & Amiot, 1960). Based on statistical analyses they concluded that the growth of the world population N is most fittingly approximated by the following hyperbolic equation.

N=C/(t_0-t)

where C and t0 are constants, whereas t0 corresponds to an absolute upper limit of the increase at which N ≈ ∞ (for details see also Korotayev & Malkov, 2016).
Obviously, von Förster et al. did not actually believe that the world population would become infinite within the doomsday interval t0 = A.D. 2026.87 ± 5.50 (op. cit., p.1293) but rather that the prognosticated longitudinal trend would change into a different direction before the critical calendrical value is reached. Others researchers have propounded an elongated L-shaped curve, i.e., a stabilization at a minimum positive level at ≈ 2050 A.D. (Konar, 2012; cf. Korotayev & Malkov, 2016).
We maintain that there are other factors which are much more important than Malthusian demographic developments per se. The fundamental problems of the 21st century Anthropocene (Lewis & Maslin, 2015) are primarily caused by the irrational, short-sighted, reckless, and ego-driven behaviour of the human species, viz., overconsumption and a myopic profit-oriented exploitation of the ecosystem (Fromm, 1962, 1976). In the field of behavioural economics short-sighted thinking has been studied in extenso under the header “temporal discounting” (van den Bos & McClure, 2013). In brevi, immediate rewards are preferred and outcomes in the future are regarded as less important (time-additive discounted utility function), a phenomenon which is also observable in non-human primates (Hwang, Kim, & Lee, 2009). However, there appear to be significant cross-cultural variations in “long versus short-term orientation”, a variable which, according to factor analytic computations, constitutes a basic cultural dimension (Minkov & Hofstede, 2012). Since the 1970s countless studies on “delay gratification” (Mischel, Ebbesen, & Raskoff Zeiss, 1972) have been conducted and recent neuroimaging work indicates that neuroanatomical loci associated with self-control (i.c., higher-order prefrontal executive functions) play a crucial rôle in the top-down regulation of impulsivity/temptation (i.c., nucleus accumbes) (Luerssen, Gyurak, Ayduk, Wendelken, & Bunge, 2014). Sufficient self-control capacity is thus a neurocognitive condicio sine qua non for successful long-term strategies. However, our current capitalistic “economic” system is primarily based on marketing principles which systematically create desires and exploit impulsivity, e.g., methods of “nagging psychology” which explicitly target developing children, and, more recently, highly effective “neuromarketing” techniques which monitor and scrutinise the fine-grained effects of various advertising strategies on neuronal processes in the brain (Stanton, Sinnott-Armstrong, & Huettel, 2017). It could be argued that prefrontal executive control circuitry (which is necessary for reflective rational thought) is systematically compromised by various methods which target and exploit dopaminergic/limbic hedonic processes (cf. Olds & Milner, 1954). According to resource models of self-control (e.g., Baumeister, Vohs, & Tice, 2007) the inherently limited capacity to regulate impulses and desires needs to be practiced (the so called “muscle analogy of self-control”, but see Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). By contrast, present Western society is highly seductive (Biehl-Missal & Saren, 2012) and it reinforces a “mindless consumption mindset” (cf. Williams & Grisham, 2012) which is combined with wasteful production principles such as “planned obsolescence” (Guiltinan, 2009) in order to stimulate economic growth (one of the explicit maxims of contemporary macroeconomics). There exists a ubiquitous focus on material externalities which is evidenced by the abundance of consumer objects. This observation is associated with a much more general ascertainment, namely, the widespread dominance of a thought-pattern which is primarily quantitative, materialistic, outward oriented (extrospective), and egocentric, as opposed to qualitative, inward directed (introspective), and unitive (the psychoanalyst Erich Fromm emphasised the fundamental difference between the “having mode” and the “being mode” of existence in much of his work, e.g., op. cit.). We argue that this imbalance is reflective of a “subject versus object” consumption dichotomy and a “separationist Weltanschauung” which conceptualises nature as an objectifiable and exploitable resource which is perceived as entirely detached from the self. This perceptual paradigm is based on a purely materialistic and mechanistic conceptualisation of biology which leaves no room for qualitative aspects like purpose and meaning (i.e., télos & lógos) (cf. “hormic” theoretical approaches to psychology, e.g., McDougall, 1930; Wolman, 1981). It is principally based on utilitarian premises which do not incorporate human values, ethics, and morality. Terms like “sustainability” and “responsibility” are strategically employed by the massive public relations industry as described by Orwell in the context of “newspeak”, i.e., psycholinguistic semantic inversion techniques are widely utilised as a façade (see also Chakravartty & Schiller, 2010; Wals & Jickling, 2002). In sum, these psycho-socio-economic factors destroy nature, antagonise rationality and foster egocentrism, irrationality, and short-sighted thinking.
The present modus operandi is congruent with the destructive philosophy of neoliberalism1F2F2F – a doctrine which is highly influential amongst the “financial power élite” (Harvey, 2007; Hill & Kumar, 2009). The continuation of the current irrational course of action will predictably lead to total ecological catastrophe in the foreseeable future (Ceballos et al., 2017) unless humanity comes up with a radical creative solution.
Due to inherent species-specific cognitive and epistemological limitations prescience is intrinsically bound. Human beings cannot possibly know the exact threshold of the “multifactorial nonlinear complex systems equation” which determines the trajectory of our habitat in state-space (which might indeed be a multidimensional Hilbert space). There are simply to many unknown variables and deterministic predictions by a hypothetical omniscient “Laplacian Demon” might be in principle impossible due to the intrinsically stochastic nature of fundamental physics (cf. recent experimental falsifications of local realism, e.g., Gröblacher et al., 2007). However, it is a plausible cautious “Bayesian prior” to assume that once a critical tipping-point is reached the system loses its equilibrium state and a “singularity” ensues (not to be confused with the dystopian AI singularity advertised by prominent advocates of transhumanism). The grand concept of the fine-tuned universe (Holder, 2002) can be generalised to the Earth System. Human beings can only exist in a very narrow band of fine-tuned environmental parameters (the “Goldilocks” zone of sapient life). Within an evolutionary blink of an eye humankind has changed the natural order (the Humboldtian Kosmos) for rather dubious reasons. Irrational primitive psychological motives like cupidity and imperiousness provide the primary impetus for this pervasive development. Sapientiæ & virtutis appear to play a subordinate rôle (if any) in this scenario which is often depicted as progress. Our self-image (as a species) is hubristic, unrealistic, and illusionary. To come vis-à-vis with the truth about our current condition might be the most difficult task which is prevented by constant distraction and various self-deceptive psychological defence mechanisms (e.g., quasi-Freudian suppression at the level of mass psychology).
In 2018, the figurative “Doomsday Clock” maintained by the BULLETIN OF ATOMIC SCIENTISTS was set to “2 minutes to midnight”. According to this heuristic multicriterial assessment, humanity was never that close to annihilation since 1953 when the USA tested the first hydrogen bomb (Guglielmi, 2018). Ergo, creativity and a fundamentally new way of thinking are of utmost evolutionary importance if the species Homō sapiēns sapiēns wants to survive this century. A creative solution to this far-reaching existential problem is thus literally a matter of life or death. As Einstein put it in a New York Times interview: “… a new type of thinking is essential if mankind is to survive and move toward higher levels.” One of the key components of the solution is a deep first-person emotive comprehension that the earth is a single complex system – an interconnected whole to which we as human beings belong.
Deeply intertwined with socio-political factors, there are systematic psychological barriers which impede rational change. For instance, the “status quo bias” which refers to an unconscious (System 1) preference for the current state of affairs (which is perceived as a comparative baseline level). Any change which deviates from this reference anchor is perceived as a loss (cf. loss-aversion, regret avoidance, existence bias, mere exposure effect, i.a.), despite the fact that an alternative course of action may be objectively advantageous (Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1991). From a social group-dynamics perspective, individuals who challenge the perceived dominant status quo (which constitutes an implicit group norm) are socially punished by the in-group (e.g., loss of reputation, discredititation, ostracism, etc.). In this way, the status quo is socially “cemented” (consolidated), and possible alternatives are prevented. It is noteworthy that norm-defection is socially contagious (for a pertinent historical Nazi Germany case-study see Geerling, Magee, & Brooks, 2015). Based on recent fMRI neuroimaging data it has been concluded that “specific prefrontal-basal ganglia dynamics are involved in rejecting the default, a mechanism that may be important in a range of difficult choice scenarios” (S. M. Fleming, Thomas, & Dolan, 2010, p. 6005). The experimental study documented a selective increase in neuronal activity in the subthalamic nucleus (STN) when the status quo was rejected. Further, statistical analysis indicated that there was a correlative increase in activity in the inferior frontal cortex for difficult decisions (as compared to easy defaults). In line with other conceptually related experimental studies, the results indicated that the frontal cortex has a modulatory top-down influence on the STN during switches away from the status quo (i.e., the non-default option). Such neuropsychophysiological data might provide important insights how to overcome the persistent status quo bias and the results indicate the importance of inhibitory cognitive control mechanisms (cf. studies on self-control/resource models of executive prefrontal functions).8F9F9F We suggest that social group pressure (e.g., conformity à la Asch) should be systematically investigated as an additional variable in future behavioural and neuroimaging studies along these lines. The problem how to overcome the detrimental status quo is of utmost importance and governmental institutions and funding bodies should acknowledge the significance and urgency of this “wicked problem” (DeFries & Nagendra, 2017) which has far-reaching ramifications for the species as a whole. The status quo bias relates to the prefatory quotation by Abraham Malow who refers to Hans-Christian Andersons fable entitled “The Emperor’s New Clothes” (published in 1837). However, what Anderson and Maslow do not mention is that the child whose innocent veridical perception of reality challenges the status quo will by all likelihood be severely punished, verbally abused, and verboten by those who proudly escort the naked emperor and praise the non-existent dress most profusely (i.e., those who benefit from the corrupt status quo, who identify with it, whose self-esteem depends on it, and who by no means want to see the truth). Thus, the child will painfully learn the consequences of not conforming to fallacious socially shared beliefs (via Bandura-type operant conditioning). Ritualism and orthopraxy (Ellul, 1973) are additional pertinent concepts in this situation. Moreover, this hypothetical punitive scenario does not include the reaction of the royal propagandists and the king himself.
In general, people do not readily give up core beliefs (Weltanschauungen) in the light of new evidence (a quasi-Bayesian epistemological desideratum). Per contrast, humans defend elementary belief tenets which fundamentally (axiomatically) structure their perspective on reality, consciously and/or unconsciously, and one might argue that Hebbian principles of long-term potentiation provide a neuronal “morphometric” explanation for cognitively inflexible “belief networks” (i.e., consolidation/dominance of specific neuronal pathways — any deviation is associated with computational outcome uncertainty). “Belief bias” (Evans, Over, & Manktelow, 1993) is one mechanism which works at the automatic/implicit level, i.e., the syntactical logical validity of a given syllogistic argument is frequently neglected and conclusions are primarily judged based on their semantic congruence with certain a priori beliefs. Around 2000 years ago, Lucius Seneca summarised this irrational human tendency concisely in one sentence: “Every man prefers belief to the exercise of judgment.” The elaboration likelihood model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986) provides a dual-process framework which distinguishes between two modes of information processing — a peripheral (heuristic) mode vs. a central (analytic) mode. The latter constitutes the reflective and rational mode of reasoning while the former is reflexive and belief based. “Rational intelligence” (RQ; Stanovich & West, 2008) is an individual differences variable that refers to people’s propensity to utilise critical thinking skills and analytic cognitive processes (note that RQ ≠ IQ). If conditions for rational/analytic processing are not met the status quo is passively adopted as the effortless default (i.e., cognitively economic heuristic decisions which do not engage any elaboration or System 2 intervention and are cost-effective in prefrontal energetic terms). Characterological idiosyncrasies and group conformity propensities are additional significant factors in this regard.
In connection to “herd psychology” the eugenist Sir Francis Galton formulated the following quasi-Darwinian argument on behavioural/cognitive/attitudinal conformity in his book “Inquiries into the Human Faculty” (1983, p. 51 et seq., cap.: ”Gregarious and Slavish Instincts”):

“An incapacity of relying on oneself and a faith in others are precisely the conditions that compel brutes to congregate and live in herds; and, again, it is essential to their safety in a country infested by large carnivora, that they should keep closely together in herds. No ox grazing alone could live for many days unless he were protected, far more assiduously and closely than is possible to barbarians. […] If any brute in a herd makes itself obnoxious to the leader, the leader attacks him, and there is a free fight between the two, the other animals looking on the while. But if a man makes himself obnoxious to his chief, he is attacked, not by the chief single-handed, but by the overpowering force of his executive. The rebellious individual has to brave a disciplined host; there are spies who will report his, doings, a local authority who will send a detachment of soldiers to drag him to trial; there are prisons ready built to hold him, civil authorities wielding legal powers of stripping him of all his possessions, and official executioners prepared to torture or kill him. The tyrannies under which men have lived, whether under rude barbarian chiefs, under the great despotisms of half-civilised Oriental countries, or under some of the more polished but little less severe governments of modern days must have had a frightful influence in eliminating independence of character from the human race.”

Much earlier, in 1851, the charismatic German philosopher Arthur Schopenhauer wrote the following on the detrimental effects of conformity and “public opinion”:

“When we see that almost everything men devote their lives to attain, sparing no effort and encountering a thousand toils and dangers in the process, has, in the end, no further object than to raise themselves in the estimation of others; when we see that not only offices, titles, decorations, but also wealth, nay, even knowledge and art, are striven for only to obtain, as the ultimate goal of all effort, greater respect from one’s fellowmen, — is not this a lamentable proof of the extent to which human folly can go? To set much too high a value on other people’s opinion is a common error everywhere; an error, it may be, rooted in human nature itself, or the result of civilization, and social arrangements generally; but, whatever its source, it exercises a very immoderate influence on all we do, and is very prejudicial to our happiness. We can trace it from a timorous and slavish regard for what other people will say, up to the feeling which made Virginius plunge the dagger into his daughter’s heart, or induces many a man to sacrifice quiet, riches, health and even life itself, for posthumous glory. Undoubtedly this feeling is a very convenient instrument in the hands of those who have the control or direction of their fellowmen; and accordingly we find that in every scheme for training up humanity in the way it should go, the maintenance and strengthening of the feeling of honor occupies an important place. […] There was much the same kind of thing in the case of Lecompte, who was executed at Frankfurt, also in 1846, for an attempt on the king’s life. At the trial he was very much annoyed that he was not allowed to appear, in decent attire, before the Upper House; and on the day of the execution it was a special grief to him that he was not permitted to shave. It is not only in recent times that this kind of thing has been known to happen. Mateo Aleman tells us, in the Introduction to his celebrated romance, Juzman de Alfarache, that many infatuated criminals, instead of devoting their last hours to the welfare of their souls, as they ought to have done, neglect this duty for the purpose of preparing and committing to memory a speech to be made from the scaffold. ” (Schopenhauer, 1851, p. 46, et seq., cap. IV “Position, or A Man’s Place in the Estimation of Others”).

In conclusio, these quotations demonstrate the restrictive impact of public doxa (Bourdieu, 1977), impression management, social desirability, and the fundamental “need to belong” (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) on independent thought which is an essential prerequisite for creativity and modifications of the status quo.

The “single-state fallacy” (Roberts, 2006, p. 104) pertains to the widely held naïve belief that worthwhile cognition exclusively takes place in “normal” alert waking consciousness — a superficial assumption which fits into the dominant contemporary materialistic and utilitarian “production habitus” which places great emphasis on “ordinary” states of consciousness and socially discriminates against “altered” states of consciousness as being unimportant, irrational, prejudicial, libidinous, and even infantile (cf. Fromm, 1976). Per contra, there exists copious evidence that important path-breaking creative ideas can emerge from “non-ordinary” states of consciousness (Tart, 1972, 2008). A well-documented paradigmatic historical example is August Kekulés discovery of the benzene structure in 1858, a landmark in the history of science which heralded the birth of the structural theory of organic chemistry (Kekulé, 1866, 1890). Kekulé, a German chemist, had a daydream of the Ouroboros (an ancient symbol of a snake seizing its own tail).10F10FThis dream-image provided him with the idea of the cyclic structure of benzene (Gillis, 1966; Rocke, 2015), i.e., a symmetrical ring comprised of six carbon atoms with alternating single and double bonds. The far-reaching scientific ramifications of Kekulés insight for the rapid development of modern chemistry can hardly be overstated. Interestingly, the Swiss depth-psychologist C.G. Jung assigned particular archetypal and alchemical significance to this ancient symbol which can be found in numerous cultural traditions across various epochs and locations (Jung, 1969). Jung wrote:

“The dream is a little hidden door in the innermost and most secret recesses of the soul, opening into that cosmic night which was psyche long before there was any ego consciousness, and which will remain psyche no matter how far our ego-consciousness extends. For all ego-consciousness is isolated; because it separates and discriminates, it knows only particulars, and it sees only those that can be related to the ego. Its essence is limitation, even though it reach to the farthest nebulae among the stars. All consciousness separates; but in dreams we put on the likeness of that more universal, truer, more eternal man dwelling in the darkness of primordial night. There he is still the whole, and the whole is in him, indistinguishable from nature and bare of all egohood. It is from these all-uniting depths that the dream arises, be it never so childish, grotesque, and immoral.” (Jung, 1933, p. 304)

Jung’s mentor, Sigmund Freud, famously characterized dreams as “the royal road to the unconscious” (Freud, 1939). However, unbeknownst to early Freudian psychoanalysts, besides dreams, parapraxis, and free-association techniques, there exist other much more effective methods to render unconscious psychic contents more accessible. Certain neuroactive chemical substances, colloquially termed “psychedelics”, are particularly potent tools in this regard. From a psychoanalytic perspective, it is noteworthy that psychedelics produce dream-like effects and may also be classified as oneirogenic substances (i.e., substances that produce or enhance dream-like states of consciousness which is of pertinence for modern oneirology). There is a significant amount of anecdotal significant evidence that psychedelics can, inter alia, enhance creative ideation (indeed the term “ideagens” has been suggested; Roberts, 2006).
From a purely pragmatic vantage point on creativity the crucial importance of psychedelics in the technological development of the internet and the personal computer should be highlighted (the digital revolution). Prima vista, this might appear like a hyperbolic statement. However, there exists considerable historical evidence in support of the claim that psychedelics played a pivotal rôle in the highly creative and innovative 1960s computer-revolution which fundamentally transformed (and interconnected) the world we inhabit (see Markoff, 2005; Nelson, 1975). A similar argument could be made with respect to the development of cybernetics as an interdisciplinary meta-discipline, e.g., the Macy conferences.11F13F13F Besides the influence of psychedelics on the development of uniting (i.e., boundary dissolving) information-technologies like the world-wide-web, innumerable artists across disciplines, epochs, and cultures have been deeply inspired by transcendental experiences occasioned by psychedelics, especially within the branch termed “visionary arts” (e.g., Grey, 2001). Indeed, it has been argued that unconscious processes play a pivotal rôle in artistic expression (e.g., Kandel, 2015). For reasons of space and parsimony we omit a discussion of this extremely rich area.
Eminent contemporary instances of “psychedelically-inspired creativity” include, for example, the entrepreneur Steve Jobs and Nobel laureate Karry Mullis. Jobs famously reported that his experience with Lysergic Acid Diethylamide (LSD) was one of the most important things he did in his whole life, a statement which recently gained experimental empirical support. Biochemist Karry Mullis was even more explicit (Mullis was honoured for his ground-breaking work on the polymerase chain reaction which is currently widely used to replicate DNA). He stated in an interview: “Back in the 1960s and early ’70s I took plenty of LSD. A lot of people were doing that in Berkeley back then. And I found it to be a mind-opening experience. It was certainly much more important than any courses I ever took” (Schoch, 1994). Mullis claimed that his ability to “get down with the molecules” was facilitated by LSD (Slattery, 2015). Moreover, he wrote in his autobiography: “The concept that there existed chemicals with the ability to transform the mind, to open up new windows of perception, fascinated me” (Mullis, 2000, p. 62). Mullis articulation reverberates with the title of Aldous Huxley’s influential book “The Doors of Perception” (Huxley, 1954) in which Huxley details his extraordinary experiences with the ancient psychedelic compound mescaline (3,4,5-trimethoxyphenethylamine) which was administered to him by the British psychiatrist Humphrey Osmond who initially coined the term “psychedelic”. Huxley wrote the following couplet in a letter to Osmond:
“To make this mundane world sublime,
Take half a gram of phanerothyme”
Osmond likewise responded to Huxley in poetic rhyme form:
“To fathom Hell or soar angelic,
Just take a pinch of psychedelic”
Huxley adopted the title of his mescaline-inspired book from a phrase found in William Blake’s 1793 poem “The Marriage of Heaven and Hell”. Blake wrote: “If the doors of perception were cleansed every thing would appear to man as it is, Infinite. For man has closed himself up, till he sees all things thro’ narrow chinks of his cavern.” According to Huxley and Blake, the transcendence of the self-centred perspective which is associated with rigid ego-structures enables the percipient to perceive reality in “new light” and from a more impartial and universal perspective. Likewise, Schopenhauer wrote the following in 1831 in a chapter entitled “Genius and Virtue”:

“The man who is devoted to knowledge of this character is not employed in the business of the will [ego]. Nay, every man who is devoted to the purely objective contemplation of the world (and it is this that is meant by the knowledge of ideas) completely loses sight of his will and its objects, and pays no further regard to the interests of his own person, but becomes a pure intelligence free of any admixture of will.” (Schopenhauer, 1831; content in bracket added).

The transcendence of psychologically conditioned habitual (aprioristic/automatic) self-centred perceptual schemata is crucial with respect to creative cognition. Psychedelics are unique in this regard because they are highly effective neurochemical tools which profoundly change perception and reveal states of consciousness that lie far beyond ordinary waking “states”. Moreover, these pharmacological agents possess the ability to catalyse the most “extraordinary” psychological phenomena known to science, e.g., transcendence of experiential space-time, synesthæsia/somasthæsia/ideasthæsia, spectacular visual hallucinations/illusions,18F20F20F ineffable imaginations/phantasmagoria, indescribable feelings of awe, intense emotional catharsis, out-of-body experiences, expansion of consciousness, phenomenological access to higher dimensions of being, experiential access to unconscious/archetypal contents, profound noetic insights, enhanced biophilia, amplified empathy and compassion, etc. pp.
In the context at hand, one of the most important qualities of these chemically well-defined compounds is their ability to catalyse novel cognitions and perceptions and their capacity to induce the process of ego-dissolution(Carhart-Harris, Erritzoe, Haijen, Kaelen, & Watts, 2018; J. V. Davis & Canty, 2013; Millière, 2017; Nour, Evans, Nutt, & Carhart-Harris, 2016), viz., the experience of nondual consciousness. In a “state” of nondual consciousness habitual categorical dichotomies which ordinarily structure all experience are dissolved. For instance, the duality between subject and object, psyche and physis, epistemology and otology, knower and known, inside and outside, percipient and perceived, self and other, ingroup and outgroup, good and bad, et cetera. With reference to recent neuroimaging research it has been eloquently stated that nondual consciousness is “a background awareness that precedes conceptualization and intention and that can contextualize various perceptual, affective, or cognitive contents without fragmenting the field of experience into habitual dualities” (Josipovic, 2014). The discussion of nondual consciousness has an extensive history in various ancient contemplative knowledge traditions (for example in India where it is associated with sophisticated yogic techniques which place great emphasis on self-control). Nonduality has only very recently become a topic in the neurosciences, and we are unaware of any controlled research which explicitly connects nondual consciousness with creativity. We submit that interdisciplinary research along this line of thought would be both light-bearing and fruitful (in the Baconian sense of lucifera & fructifera).
Among numerous experts in the field of psychedelic research, there exists general consensus that psychedelics (i.e., consciousness expanding substances) can augment cognitive processes and enable states of “unconstrained cognition” (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012; cf. Sheldrake, McKenna, Abraham, & Abraham, 2001). Therefore, it is argued that psychedelics are important neurochemical research tools that can significantly broaden our understanding of creativity. However, this idea is not new. An early pilot study from the 1960s (which is by modern research standards methodologically confounded/unsound) indicated that psychedelics can significantly enhance creativity and rational scientific problem solving (Harman, McKim, Mogar, Fadiman, & Stolaroff, 1966). After an initial phase of systematic scientific research, the legal prohibition of psychedelics in the late 1960s put an abrupt halt to the short-lived but very promising research agenda, primarily due to the questionable “War on Drugs” which was initiated by the Nixon administration for evidently perfidious/ominous motives.
After an elongated legally enforced research-hiatus, science is currently witnessing a “psychedelic renaissance”, a new rising wave of psychedelic research (Bolstridge, 2013; Cameron & Olson, 2018; Roseman, Demetriou, Wall, Nutt, & Carhart-Harris, 2018; Sessa, 2012) using modern psychological methodologies and advanced neuroimaging technologies (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Roseman et al., 2016; Tagliazucchi et al., 2016). One can only speculate how far science would have progressed in this domain without the inhibiting effects of the judicially imposed interregnum. Hitherto systematic scientific research which focuses specifically on the rôle of psychedelics on creativity is virtually absent22F24F24F and the compound 5-MeO-DMT has to date not been investigated in a rigorous manner. In connection to our previous argument on nonduality and creativity, we predict that future research along these lines will be very probative. Research on psychedelic agents is especially pertinent for our understanding of the neuroscience of creativity because many psychedelics have endogenous counterparts, in other terms, they are structurally similar or identical to neurotransmitters which constitute human physiology/neurochemistry.
Many neuroscientists are unaware that the discovery of LSD-25 led to the idea that neurochemicals might play a central rôle in cognitive processes (Cozzi, 2013). Today the fact that neurotransmitters influence cognition is taken for granted. However, before 1952 serotonin was thought to be a vasoconstrictor (hence the composite lexeme “sero-tonin”). In 1952-53 serotonin (5-hydroxtryptamin, 5-HT) was discovered in the brain by Betty Twarog, Irvine Page, and Sir Henry Gaddum (for a historical review see Twarog, 1988). In 1953, Sir Gaddum took LSD in a self-experiment. Shortly afterward he and his colleague published a paper on the antagonistic effects of LSD on 5-HT (Gaddum & Hameed, 1954). Gaddum conjectured a common site of action between both compounds and theorized that the neurocognitive effects of LSD result from its action on 5-HT (Amin, Crawford, & Gaddum, 1954). Because he had experienced the effects of LSD first-hand (self-experiments were quite common), he knew that it produces significant mental changes. Knowing that LSD antagonizes 5-HT, he made the novel theoretical connection for the first time in the documented history of science. That is, Gaddum was the first to postulate that 5-HT might play a rôle in cognition (Cozzi, 2013). This historical example clearly demonstrates that the systematic study of psychedelic compounds is indispensable if science wants to deepen its understanding of various psychological processes (e.g., creativity) and their neuronal correlates. We agree with other creativity researchers that “evidence gleaned from the structure and function of the brain [can] enhance our ability to foster creativity” (Vartanian, 2013, p. 257; content in brackets added). We propound that the systematic scientific exploration of the yet uninvestigated endogenous compound 5-MeO-DMT may provide important novel insight into the neural correlates of its currently only vaguely and anecdotally characterised psychological and phenomenological effects. This type of innovative research has the potential to foster our basic understanding of the evolutionary functions of various tryptamines in human consciousness. Further, this research agenda may lead to novel psychopharmacological interventions and aid in the elucidation of hitherto unidentified neurotransmitter systems (cf. the pathbreaking discovery of the endogenous cannabinoid system which heralded a new and rapidly growing field in medicine). In addition, 5-MeO-DMTs molecular structure could be systematically varied (cf. A. A. Shulgin & Shulgin, 1997) in order to rigorously explore structure–activity relationships. Such research might in theory lead to the discovery of “super-agonists” (Langmead & Christopoulos, 2013). The exploration of synergistic effects with other naturally occurring psychoactive substances (e.g., Ibogaine (Barsuglia et al., 2018; Glick & Maisonneuve, 1998; Winkelman, 2015)) is another hitherto uncharted and potentially very fruitful research area. In addition, allosteric modulators are of great scientific interest in this respect (cf. Schwartz & Holst, 2007). That is, the agonistic actions of 5-MeO-DMT can in principle be enhanced (>100% efficacy) by various allosteric modulators (e.g., via allosteric modulators of G protein–coupled receptors; cf. May, Leach, Sexton, & Christopoulos, 2007). Yet another related important research question concerns the “entourage effect” (cf. Sanchez-Ramos, 2015). The appurtenant open research question is: What are the neuropsychopharmacological and phenomenological differences between the pure compound (5-MeO-DMT synthesised in the laboratory) and the compound as found in nature, i.e., within a whole complex biologism organism (toad venom, tree bark, seed pods, etc.)?
In order to provide corroborating empirical evidence for our hypothesis that psychedelics are important research tools in regard to creativity research, we will now discuss two contemporary experimental studies which are pertinent to the psychology and neuroscience of creativity. Based on the relevant literature (e.g., Nour et al., 2016), we specifically argue that an understanding of the psychological and neurophysiological processes which undergird ego-dissolution (i.e., nonduality) is pivotal for advancing our scientific understanding of creativity. After introducing the corroborating studies, we will provide more detailed information on the underappreciated and virtually unresearched endogenously occurring compound 5-MeO-DMT. Based on this empirical background we will then formulate several empirically falsifiable hypotheses (the main hypothesis is presented in from of a concise and logically valid syllogistic argument).

Psilocybin increases the personality-trait “Openness to Experience”
Psilocybin (O-phosphoryl-4-hydroxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine) is an indole alkaloid (a structural relative of 5-MeO-DMT)25F27F27F first synthesized and named by the Swiss chemist Albert Hofmann26F28F28F (Hofmann et al., 1959; 1958). After the elucidation of its chemical properties the active principle, psilocin (4-hydroxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine), was identified (Hofmann & Troxler, 1959). Various experimental chemical modifications were made to both structures (Troxler, Seemann, & Hofmann, 1959). The psilocybin molecule is present in more than 200 fungal species which span numerous taxa, some of which are endemic to the USA and Europe, e.g., Psilocybe semilanceata (colloquially known as “Liberty Cap”). In cross-cultural shamanic contexts, psilocybin has been reportedly utilized for spiritual and healing purposes for millennia (Akers, Ruiz, Piper, & Ruck, 2011; Hofmann, Heim, Brack, & Kobel, 1958). Its molecular structure closely resembles 5-hydroxtryptamine (5-HT, serotonin). In humans, psilocybin functions as a prodrug and is rapidly dephosphorylated to psilocin which acts as a non-selective partial 5-HT receptor agonist. It shows particularly high binding affinity for the 5-HT1A, 5-HT2A, and 5-HT2C receptor subtypes (Kraehenmann et al., 2015; Nichols, 2004).
A landmark study conducted at Johns Hopkins University by MacLean, Johnson, & Griffiths (2011) experimentally demonstrated that a single high-dose of psilocybin can induce long-lasting personality changes in the domain “Openness to Experience”, as measured by the widely used NEO-PI (Personality Inventory). Openness to Experience (OTE) is one of the core dimensions of the extensively employed quinquepartite (big five) model of personality (i.e., factor V). OTE is an amalgamation of several interconnected personality traits which include: 1) aesthetic appreciation and sensitivity, 2) fantasy and imagination, 3) awareness of feelings in self and others, and 4) intellectual engagement, inter alia. Most relevant for the context at hand is the fact that OTE has a strong and reliable correlation with creativity (Ivcevic & Brackett, 2015; S. B. Kaufman et al., 2016; Silvia, Nusbaum, Berg, Martin, & O’Connor, 2009). Individuals with high scores on the OTE dimension are “permeable to new ideas and experiences” and “motivated to enlarge their experience into novel territory” (DeYoung, Peterson, & Higgins, 2005). Furthermore, OTE is associated with personality trait “absorption” (Glisky, Tataryn, Tobias, Kihlstrom, & McConkey, 1991) and we argue that absorption is a state of primordial awareness akin to nondual consciousness. That is, absorption is a type of awareness in which the dichotomy between the sensor and sensed, the percipient and perceived, the seer and seen, transiently dissolves into a state of union which is devoid of any intermediary imagistic, symbolic, or linguistic cognitive preconceptions which usually intercede between the experience and the experienced (i.e., the thing “in itself” and the experienced percept). It is therefore an undistorted state of pure and total awareness without any abstraction and without a Kantian a priori space-time interval which generally mediates between subject and object and therefore pre-structures the field of experience in a dualistic format. “Boundaries of the mind” is another conceptually related trait associated with OTE and creativity. It relates to the boundaries between fantasy and reality, sleeping and waking, self and other, and furthermore, the “permeability of ego boundaries” (Hartmann, Rosen, & Rand, 1998; McCrae, 1994).
In the described study by MacLean et al. (op cit.) the psilocybin-induced increase in OTE was mediated by the intensity of the mystical experience. Importantly, ego-dissolution is a central feature of mystical experiences (see also Griffiths, Richards, McCann, & Jesse, 2006) and we argue that ego-dissolution can culminate in the peak experience of nonduality. Based on this evidence, we hypothesize that the experience of ego-dissolution (viz., nonduality) predicts post eventum increases in creativity and we postulate a causal relationship between factors. We term this the “less ego => more creativity hypothesis”. Furthermore, we argue that OTE is an important factor relating to the status quo bias discussed before. OTE is associated with explorative behaviour, novelty seeking, and cognitive flexibility. From a neuroscientific perspective the status quo bias may be based on Hebbian principles of long-term potentiation (LTP). That is, repeatedly utilized neural circuits are diachronically strengthened (Hebb, 1949) and become dominant and rigid. The social environment may thus entrain the “Hebbian status quo”, i.e., the social milieu consolidates specific neuronal circuitry (via social conditioning, education, enculturation, etc.). Interestingly, complex system theory suggests a bipolar (orthogonal) continuum ranging from rigidity on one end to chaos on the other. Integration lies interjacent between the extremes. Given that the cognitive system can be regarded as a complex system, this generic account might lend itself to conceptualise a “cognitive continuum of information processing states” (Faust & Kenett, 2014) ranging from rigid cognition to chaotic cognition (i.e., closed-mindedness to open-mindedness). In a rigid neural network, nodes are only sparsely interconnected (i.e., cognitive hyper-rigidity). In a chaotic neural network topology, on the other hand, virtually all nodes are interconnected (i.e., cognitive over-flexibility/chaos). According to this schematic, cognitive integration (viz., the linkage of differentiated parts (Siegel, 2010)) is characterised by an intermediate neuronal network connectivity pattern which balances and synchronizes the polar extremes (i.e., adaptive/dynamic cognitive coherence). We argue that the balance between neuronal differentiation and integration is of pertinence with respect to creativity research and the subsequent section provides further details on this proposal, i.e., specifically with respect to the effects of LSD-25 on brain-wide connectivity. In support of this quasi-Lockean “associationist/connectivist” idea, recent neuroimaging work has correlated OTE with increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks (Passamonti et al., 2014). It would therefore be of great interest to explicitly test the hypothesis that increases in OTE (experimentally induced by psychedelics such as, e.g., psilocybin, LSD, or 5-MeO-DMT) are mediated by increases in functional connectivity in specific networks, i.e., specifically those associated with introspection, self-control, and self-referential processing (cf. Parkinson, Kornelsen, & Smith, 2019; Smigielski, Scheidegger, Kometer, & Vollenweider, 2019).

LSD expands global functional connectivity density in the brain
LSD-25 is one of the most potent psychedelic compounds known to science, producing profound alterations of consciousness after submilligram oral doses ≥20 µg (Nichols, 2018a). A recent multimodal fMRI study by Tagliazucchi et al. (2016) conducted at Imperial College London administered LSD intravenously to healthy volunteers. The researchers found that LSD-induced ego-dissolution was statistically significantly correlated with an increase in global functional connectivity density (FCD) between various brain networks, indicating that the psychedelic enabled novel configurations of brain states. As discussed in the previous study by MacLean et al. (2011), mystical experience (i.c., ego-dissolution) is correlated with an increase in OTE (which in turn is strongly correlated with creativity). One of the key findings of the LSD/fMRI-study was that high-level cortical regions and the thalamus displayed increased connectivity under the acute influence of the psychedelic. In concreto, increased global activity was observed bilaterally in the high-level association cortices and the thalamus (often regarded as the brains “central information hub” which relays information between various subcortical areas and the cerebral cortices). The global activity increase in the higher-level areas partially overlapped with the default-mode, salience, and frontoparietal attention networks. The FCD changes in the default-mode and salience network were predicted a priori due their association with self-consciousness. As predicted, a significant correlation between subjectively reported ego-dissolution and an increase in global connectivity between networks was detected. The results of this milestone study demonstrate for the first time that LSD increases global inter-module connectivity (while at the same time decreasing the integrity of individual modules). Specifically, LSD enhanced the connectivity between normally separated brain networks (as quantified by the widely used Φ connectivity/associativity index). The observed changes in activity significantly correlated with the anatomical distribution of 5-HT2A receptors. We argue that these findings are highly relevant for the identification of the neural correlates of creativity because it is reasonable to postulate that an enhanced communication répertoire between previously disconnected neuronal network modules is crucial for the generation of novel ideas (cf. D. W. Moore et al., 2009). Moreover, associative processes are generally assumed to play a key rôle in creativity (C. S. Lee, Huggins, & Therriault, 2014) and a recent fMRI study provided corroborating evidence for the assumption that greater functional connectivity is related to the capacity to generate novel ideas (Beaty et al., 2018). The researchers argued that “the ability to simultaneously engage the default, executive, and salience brain systems may provide a neurophysiological marker of creative thinking ability”. Tagliazucchi et al. concluded that LSD reorganizes the rich-club architecture of brain networks and that this restructuring is accompanied by a shift of the boundaries between self and environment. That is, the ego-based dichotomy (i.e., dualism) between self and other, subject and object, internal and external, dissolves as a function of specific connectivity changes in the modular networks of the brain. In conclusio, Tagliazucchi et al. (2016) demonstrated that LSD induced ego-dissolution is accompanied by significant changes in neuronal rich-club architecture and that ego-dissolution is accompanied by the downregulation of the default-mode network (DMN). Pertaining to creativity research this finding is particularly intriguing because the DMN may be associated with habitual thought and behaviour patterns (Beucke et al., 2014; Koçak, Kale, & Çiçek, 2012). We suggest that downregulation of the DMN by psychedelics (which is accompanied by the phenomenology of ego-dissolution) is an important component for understanding the functional connectome which undergirds creativity. Based on these findings, we propose a novel neuropsychopharmacological mechanism for the enhancement of creativity which has, to our best knowledge, never been proposed before. Our hypothesis highlights the importance of ego-dissolution for the enhancement of creativity. That is, a reduction of the influence of self-referential ego structures (ex hypothesi mediated via DMN disintegration) on perception and cognition enables perspectival multiplicity and cognitive flexibility which is crucial for creative ideation. Based on the conjecture that ego-dissolution provides a “cognitive reset” which enables human beings to perceive and conceptualize reality from a more unconstrained (and ultimately nondualistic) perspective, we argue that 5-MeO-DMT is an especially intriguing molecule in this regard because its ego-dissolving effects are much more pronounced than those of psilocybin or LSD (or in fact any other known psychedelic). The “reset theory” is a first primitive attempt to formulate a causal mechanism which could explain why ego dissolution is associated with the hypothesized increase in creativity. Ego-dissolution could enable humans to “see things with new eyes” — i.e., via a reduction of the structuring and organizing influence of perceptual schemata (i.e., preconception vs. apperception). Empirical data indicates that ego-dissolution is a unique property of psychedelic substances (Nour et al., 2016). In a web-based study utilizing the “Ego-Dissolution Inventory” (EDI) several psychoactive substances were compared, and the results showed that only psychedelics were significantly correlated with the experience of ego dissolution. Per contra, other psychoactive substance like alcohol or cocaine enhance an egoic style of cognition (ego inflation). In the same study, participants also responded to a subset of items from the “Mystical Experiences Questionnaire” (MEQ) (Barrett, Johnson, & Griffiths, 2015). The results indicated a positive correlation between psychedelic dose and the strength of the mystical experience. As discussed, a defining feature of the mystical experience is an ego-dissolving “unitive” (nondual) experience. In other terms, the noetic insight that human beings are ultimately all connected via consciousness (which is singular and not plural, i.e., the apparent multiplicity is a superficial phenomenon and consciousness is in actuality not dividable). This topic has already been addressed by William James more than a century ago (James, 1985/1902). Unity experience is closely related to the Freudian concept of “oceanic feeling” (oceanic boundlessness) — a sensation of being one with universe. In fact, Romain Rolland formulated the phrase in a letter to Freud. Rolland argued that it is this nondual experience which lies at the core of all religious feelings (theistic or nontheistic). Freud utilized this idea in his later writings and hypothesized that this nondual state of consciousness is a psychological residue from the infantile stage in which the egoic schism between self and other (object and subject) has not yet occurred (Freud, 1930). That is, according to Freud, nondual experiences are a relic of the developmental stage in which the infants formation of the self-concept has not yet taken place and has consequently not yet dichotomised experience (perception) into universal “self versus non-self” dichotomies.

5-MeO-DMT: An endogenous neurochemical catalyst of creativity
According to documented history, the intranasal administration of 5-MeO-DMT in form of a snuff preparation called “Cohoba”34F36F36F by the Taíno people of Hispaniola was first observed around 1496 by Friar Ramón Pané who reported his observation to Christopher Columbus who in 1492 made initial contact with this culture (Nunn & Qian, 2010; Shultes, 1976; Torres & Repke, 2006). As regards contemporary science, 5-MeO-DMT is a relatively unknown member of a group of naturally-occurring psychoactive indolealkylamines (Glennon & Rosecrans, 1982; A. T. Shulgin & Carter, 1980). It was first synthesized by Japanese chemists in 1936 who published their results in German (Hoshino & Shimodaira, 1936). It was later extracted and isolated from Dictyoloma incanescens bark (Pachter, Zacharias, & Ribeirol, 1959), a flowering plants that belongs to the family Rutaceae. The tryptamine is an analogue of tryptophan and endogenous to human physiology. Research indicates that 5-MeO-DMT may be endogenously synthesized in human pineal and retina. Moreover, it has been detected in blood, urine, and cerebrospinal fluid (H.-W. Shen, Jiang, Winter, & Yu, 2010). Its extremely powerful acute effects are pharmacokinetically short-lived. i.e., ≈ 20-40 min (Ott, 2001). As many other tryptamine psychedelics, it acts as a nonselective 5-HT agonist and causes a broad spectrum of highly interesting psychological effects. It displays a relatively high binding affinity for the 5-HT1A, 5-HT2A , and 5-HT2C subtypes35F37F37F (Krebs-Thomson, Ruiz, Masten, Buell, & Geyer, 2006), but other mechanism of actions appear to be involved in its psychoactivity (e.g., inhibition of enzymatic monoamine oxidase activity; but see Nagai, Nonaka, & Satoh Hisashi Kamimura, 2007). The 5-HT system is associated with, cognition, emotion, and memory, inter alia. For example, 5-HT receptors are located in the cerebral cortex (cognition), in the amygdala (emotions), and in the raphe nucleus (its projection regulate circadian rhythms, alertness, inhibition of pain, inter alia). The raphe nucleus is located in the phylogenetically most primitive part of the brain, the brainstem, and its serotonergic axons project widely throughout the cortex. The raphe nucleus produces the majority of brain serotonin and it contains ≈ 85% of all the of the serotonin neurons in the brain (Hornung, 2003). Ergo, when it is stimulated by 5-MeO-DMT it causes extensive serotonergic activation throughout many interconnected neural networks. Moreover, 5-HT receptors are present in the hypothalamus which connects the central nervous system to the endocrine system. It can be cogently argued that 5-MeO-DMT causes hypothalamic release of significant amounts of the neuropeptide oxytocin via the pituitary gland. This hypothetical increase in oxytocinergic activity might explain why the qualitative linguistic descriptions of 5-MeO-DMTs phenomenology frequently include words like “love”, “unity”, and “connectedness” (these semantic descriptors are obviously very imprecise, vague, and ambiguous — a general problem of human language). Accumulating evidence indicates that 5-MeO-DMT is an endogenous ligand of the Trace amine-associated receptors (TAARs), a class of G protein-coupled receptors that were only recently discovered in 2001 (Carbonaro & Gatch, 2016). It has been hypothesized that TAARs are involved in sensory perception (Wallach, 2009). Moreover, TAARs have been associated with pathological neuroadaptations associated with prolonged exposure to addictive drugs (e.g., alcohol, heroin, cocaine, etc.). Consequently, this molecular target might partially explain 5-MeO-DMTs promising neurorestorative and neuroprotective effects (Dakic, 2017). Because 5-MeO-DMT is able to target these receptors it might be able to regulate the pathological neurological adaptions, for example those caused by various substance (and possibly behavioural) addictions (cf. the neuropsychological “reset-hypothesis” (e.g., Carhart-Harris et al., 2017)). Hence 5-MeO-DMT might counteract rigid cognitive and behavioural patterns and facilitate cognitive flexibility (cf. Gruner & Pittenger, 2017). In support of this view, a recent cutting-edge in vivo and in silico study using human cerebral organoids (Dakic et al., 2017) demonstrated that 5-MeO-DMT has modulatory effects on neuroplastic processes, long-term potentiation, cytoskeletal reorganization, and microtubule dynamics (cf. Hameroff & Penrose, 2014). Specifically, it was found that 5-MeO-matches the σ1 receptor which regulates cytoskeletal dendritic spine morphology and neurite outgrowth. Therefore, σ1 receptor agonism may potentially mediate neuroplastic processes which are crucial for creativity, cognitive flexibility, and sustained cognitive/behavioural changes (Sun et al., 2016). In addition, agonism of the σ1 receptor has been shown to have anti-inflammatory effects (Szabo, 2015) which may positively influence various creativity related cognitive processes and also genetic/cellular health, e.g., a hypothetical link between creativity, depression, inflammation, and telomeres (via telomerase activity) (see Wolkowitz et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2016). A related study recently demonstrated that 5-MeO-DMT increases in vivo adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice (Lima da Cruz, Moulin, Petiz, & Leão, 2018). The researchers administered a single dose of 5-MeO-DMT (via intracerebroventricular injection) and measured subsequent quantitative increases in cell proliferation of granule cells (GC) within the dentate gyrus (DG) of the subgranular zone of the hippocampus. Neurogenesis was accompanied by a complexification of GC dendritic morphology, i.e., more complex dendritic tress relative to controls. These findings indicate that 5-MeO-DMT can can increase neuronal survival, stimulate cell proliferation, and accelerate maturation of newborn neurons in the hippocampal DG region. Comparable results have previously been obtained with psilocybin (Catlow, Song, Paredes, Kirstein, & Sanchez-Ramos, 2013) which is suggestive of a common serotonergic mechanism of action which is causative for the observed increases in adult hippocampal neurogenesis.
5-MeO-DMT is widespread in the plant kingdom and has been used in shamanic rituals for millennia (Torres et al., 1991). While its structural relative psilocybin is exclusively present in fungi, 5-MeO-DMT is present in various plants, for instance Virola theiodora (Agurell et al., 1969), a tree species belonging to the Myristicaceae (nutmeg) family. In additions to its relatively ubiquitous phytochemical distribution, it is present in high concentrations in the venom of Incilius alvarius (known as the Sonoran Desert toad), an Amphibia which produces significant amounts of 5-Meo-DMT in its numerous parotoid glands as a defensive chemical mechanism against predators (Erspamer, Vitali, Roseghini, & Cei, 1965; Hutchinson & Savitzky, 2004). The salience of toad symbolism in Mesoamerican art and mythology is remarkable and well documented by anthropologists, for example, toad effigies (with oftentimes accentuated glands) are prominent in the historical remains of the Mayan and Aztec cultures (Davis & Weil, 1992).36F38F38F Moreover, 5-MeO-DMT is sometime used as an adjunct in certain variations of Ayahuasca (a drinkable plant-based concoction, which is utilized by indigenous tribes in the Amazonian rainforest for divinatory and healing purposes). For instance, the leaves of the plant “Chaliponga” (Diplopterys cabrerana) are occasionally added to the concoction to intensify its psychoactive effects (Callaway et al., 2006; Rätsch, 1998). Therefore, synergistic effects between 5-MeO-DMT and DMT should be systematically examined in future studies. We suggest nonlinear interactions between these compounds, i.e., the effects are not additive but multiplicative and this hypothesis should be testable using various neuroimaging techniques. Furthermore, interactions between compounds may reveal novel insights into the differential phenomenological aspects of 5-Meo-DMT and DMT which would not be possible if each compound would be investigated in isolation.
5-MeO-DMT has been utilized for spiritual purposes as a religious sacrament in the rituals of the Christian “Church of the Tree of Life” and other syncretic churches. Indeed a “bio-psycho-socio-spiritual model” has recently been proposed which is based on the persuasive premise that tryptaminergic psychedelics may have “therapeutic effects against various diseases of civilization” (Frecska, Bokor, & Winkelman, 2016). The unique transcendental phenomenology which is elicited by 5-MeO-DMT has influenced the “visionary arts”. Artworks inspired by 5-MeO-DMT experiences are oftentimes geometrically highly complex and depict multidimensional fractal-like symmetric mathematical structures37F39F39F an observation which is particularly intriguing from a neuroaesthetics point of view (cf. Ramachandran & Hirstein, 1999). Despite its longstanding usage in the course of human evolution38F40F40F , controlled human trials are currently lacking, and science knows very little about the psychological effects of 5-MeO-DMT. This research area is thus truly uncharted novel scientific territory (and its exploration required openness to experience on the part of the research community; ibid., p. 14). In line with prior related arguments (Osmond, 1957) it has recently been argued that 5-MeO-DMT is of “potential interest for schizophrenia research owing to its hallucinogenic properties” and that research on this compound can “help to understand the neurobiological basis of hallucinations” (Riga, Soria, Tudela, Artigas, & Celada, 2014).41 However, it is noteworthy that 5-MeO-DMT induced visual hallucinations are much less commonly reported compared to its structural analogue N,N-Dimethyltryptamine (DMT) which is reliably capable of inducing the most spectacular and vivid visual phenomena possibly imaginable (but see Strassman, 2001). We would like to emphasise that the current research Zeitgeist is very biased towards neuromechanistic explanations and we maintain that the most intriguing effects of psychedelics on the human psyche (i.e., consciousness) cannot be reduced to molecular mechanism and neuronal interactions. The Crickean stipulation that “a person’s mental activities are entirely due to the behaviour of nerve cells, glial cells, and the atoms, ions, and molecules that make them up and influence them” is not very plausible. In accordance with our view, Cristof Koch (who collaborated with Crick over several years in an unsuccessful attempt to solve the hard problem of consciousness in a materialistic reductionist framework) for some unknown reason changed his mind on this most fundamental topic. He wrote the following in a 2014 Scientific American article entitled “Is Consciousness Universal”:

“Yet the mental is too radically different for it to arise gradually from the physical. This emergence of subjective feelings from physical stuff appears inconceivable and is at odds with a basic precept of physical thinking, the Ur-conservation law—ex nihilo nihil fit. So if there is nothing there in the first place, adding a little bit more won’t make something. If a small brain won’t be able to feel pain, why should a large brain be able to feel the godawfulness of a throbbing toothache? Why should adding some neurons give rise to this ineffable feeling? The phenomenal hails from a kingdom other than the physical and is subject to different laws. I see no way for the divide between unconscious and conscious states to be bridged by bigger brains or more complex neurons.”

We propose that 5-MeO-DMT can help to shed new light quanta on the hard problem of consciousness which has engaged philosophers since time immemorial (neuroscience has only very recently joined the debate). Specifically, the tryptamine can provide first-hand (qualitative) insights into the ultimately nondual nature of mind and matter and the universality of consciousness — i.e., a higher perspective on human existence. In contemporary consciousness research the mind is often equated with consciousness. However, mind ≠ consciousness. This is easily verifiable because cognition (thought) is an object of consciousness (otherwise introspection on mental events would be impossible). Thus, the mind is situated within consciousness and it needs to be accentuated that science is an activity of the mind. Ergo, science is an activity within the mind which, in turn, is situated within consciousness. The statement that science can objectively investigate consciousness is thus a non sequitur. This relates to the ancient self-referential (autopoietical/recursive) question about the relationship between the seer and the seen (Sanskrit: Dṛg-Dṛśya): Can the seer be seen? Can consciousness investigate itself? Thus, the quintessential question is: Who introspects on the mind and its contents (thoughts, feelings, perceptions, etc.). In other words, who is the experiencer, who is the knower, who is the seer? Who is the ultimate conscious “I”? The 8th century Indian logician Jagadguru Shankaracharya provided the following précis of the timeless nondual philosophy:

“Even in the state of ignorance, when one sees something, through what instrument should one know That owing to which all this is known? For that instrument of knowledge itself falls under the category of objects. The knower may desire to know not about itself, but about objects. As fire does not burn itself, so the self does not know itself, and the knower can have no knowledge of a thing that is not its object. Therefore through what instrument should one know the knower owing to which this universe is known, and who else should know it? And when to the knower of Brahman who has discriminated the Real from the unreal there remains only the subject, absolute and one without a second, through what instrument, O Maitreyī, should one know that Knower?”

Nonduality is first-hand experience. It cannot be objectified. This fact does not exclude it from scientific discourse. Science is an open-ended enterprise and its methods evolve (n.b., there is no “scientific” consensus about what exactly constitutes the scientific method, e.g., the Popperian demarcation problem). By definition, science is concerned with knowledge (lat. scire = “to know”) – that is, any kind of knowledge (but see the Jamesian “radical empiricism” argument in the discussion section). Science does not preclude non-objectifiable self-knowledge. In fact, the Greek intellectual tradition (which laid the very groundwork for contemporary thought) placed great emphasis on self-knowledge as exemplified by the aphorism γνῶθι σεαυτόν (“know thyself”). In opposition to the dominant Zeitgeist, science is thus not exclusively concerned with the material aspects of existence. Paradoxically, 5-MeO-DMT provides a neurochemical (material) method which reliably induced the first-hand transcendental experience of nonduality. There are other much more arduous pathways to achieve nondual insights such as mediation and sophisticated yogic exercises. However, they are much more demanding and hence unreliable than the “direct” neurochemical route. Consequently, it would be of great interest to investigate whether these ancient techniques induce neuronal and psychological changes which are similar to those induces by 5-MeO-DMT and its structural relatives. Specifically, future studies should clarify whether the production and release of DMT-related endogenous substances can be intentionally increased by various techniques (e.g., meditation, music, yoga, physical exercise, mood manipulation, stress, pain, anxiety, mortality salience, etc.). It has been reported that average daily output of melatonin from the pineal gland is ≈ 30 μg (Nichols, 2018b). Based on this quantity it has been argued that the endogenous amount of DMT is about 1/1000 of what would be needed to achieve psychoactivity (the exact threshold is currently undefined). It would therefore be of fundamental interest to investigate if the endogenous synthesis of various tryptamines in the pineal (and elsewhere) can be stimulated, and if so, to determine the maximum output capacity of various glandular systems under varying conditions. Further, it may be assumed that there are interindividual differences in the production and release of various tryptaminergic neuro-hormones (which may be rooted in genetic variability). It is a well-established fact that there are individual differences in pineal gland volume and these have been related to autism (Maruani et al., 2019) which can be regarded as a “disorder” of consciousness. Chronobiology and various “states” of consciousness (e.g., dreaming/waking states) are closely related and tryptamines such as melatonin have been related to various mental disorders such as depression and anxiety.
5-MeO-DMT exerts extremely profound acute and chronic effects on the self-concept (ego). Here, the term ego is not used as defined in the classical Freudian tripartite model (Freud, 1923), but it refers to the concept of an encapsulated ego identity, that is, who we think we are as human beings. Thus, the usage of the term ego is more closely aligned with the ancient Sanskrit term “Ahaṃkāra” as defined in Vedic philosophy (cf. Cartesian positional identity; Comfort, 1979). For example, the great scientist of the mind Patañjali writes in Sanskrit:

दृग्दर्शनशक्त्योरेकात्मतैवास्मिता ॥६॥
“To identify consciousness with that which merely reflects consciousness – this is egoism.” (Yoga Sūtras of Patañjali, cap. 2, aphorism 6).
In the Indian contemplative tradition, the overcoming of egocentrism is a precondition for spiritual progress. The founder of quantum physics, Nobel laurate Erwin Schrödinger, was deeply impressed by the depth of Vedāntic philosophy in relation to consciousness and he expressed this eloquently in his seminal book “What is Life”:
“The only possible alternative is simply to keep the immediate that consciousness is a singular of which the plural is unknown; that there is only one thing and that, which seems to be a plurality, is merely a series of different aspects of this one thing, produced by a deception (the Indian Maya); the same illusion is produced in a gallery of mirrors, and in the same way Gaurisankar and Mt. Everest turned out to be the same peak seen from different valleys.” (Schrödinger, 1944, p. 89).

We suggest that the ancient Indian concept of Māyā is essential in order to appreciate the unveiling effects of psychedelics. Māyā connotes “that which exists, but is constantly changing and thus is spiritually unreal” (Hiriyanna, 1995). It has been roughly translated as illusion even though this translation has its shortcomings (translations from Sanskrit into English face many hermeneutical difficulties, a better twofold Vedantic translation is “projection” and “veil”). Apropos the propounded differentiation between mind and consciousness Māyā refers to the fluctuating contents of the mind.

In this theoretical/phenomenological framework, the ego can be conceptualized as a filter or a lens which converts experiences and creates the appearance of change and diversity while the underlying reality is timeless unity. Pure consciousness lies beyond the mind and the ego construct and is “that which perceives” (cf. Josipovic, 2010, 2014). While the ego identifies with the dualistic and ephemeral contents of sensory experience, consciousness itself does not (Sivananda, 1972). Consciousness itself has no associated identity. It is a detached unchanging witness of experience.40
Sir Arthur Eddington articulated similar thoughts:
“The mind-stuff of the world is, of course, something more general than our individual conscious minds. […] The mind-stuff is not spread in space and time; these are part of the cyclic scheme ultimately derived out of it. […] It is necessary to keep reminding ourselves that all knowledge of our environment from which the world of physics is constructed, has entered in the form of messages transmitted along the nerves to the seat of consciousness. […] Consciousness is not sharply defined, but fades into subconsciousness; and beyond that we must postulate something indefinite but yet continuous with our mental nature. […] It is difficult for the matter-of-fact physicist to accept the view that the substratum of everything is of mental character. But no one can deny that mind is the first and most direct thing in our experience, and all else is remote inference.” (Eddington, 1929, pp. 276–281)

More recently it has been argued along the same vein that physics faces its final frontier – i.e., consciousness (Stapp, 2007). For instance, the “von Neumann-Wigner interpretation”, also referred to as “consciousness caused collapse of Ψ”, postulates that consciousness is an essential factor in quantum measurements. Von Neumann used the term “subjective perception” (Von Neumann, 1955) which is closely related to the complementarity principle in psychophysics, viz., the complementarity of sensation & perception (J. C. Baird, 1997). Accordingly, Henry Stapp argued in his seminal paper “Quantum Theory and the Role of Mind in Nature”:

“From the point of view of the mathematics of quantum theory it makes no sense to treat a measuring device as intrinsically different from the collection of atomic constituents that make it up. A device is just another part of the physical universe. […] Moreover, the conscious thoughts of a human observer ought to be causally connected most directly and immediately to what is happening in his brain, not to what is happening out at some measuring device. […] Our bodies and brains thus become […] parts of the quantum mechanically described physical universe. Treating the entire physical universe in this unified way provides a conceptually simple and logically coherent theoretical foundation.” (Stapp, 2001)
We argue that these epistemological and ontological considerations have deep implications for the philosophy (and hence practice) of science (n.b., there is no science devoid of philosophy — only science which incorporates philosophical axioms as a priori given without explicit reflection on their validity/veridicality). The nondual perceptive challenges the predominant reductive materialism stance and it might ultimately facilitate a Kuhnian paradigm shift with far-reaching ramification across disciplines (i.e., a transdisciplinary paradigm-shift). A nondual conception of reality might enable new ways of psychophysical scientific experimentation which are unthinkable in the current materialistic paradigm (which entails a significant amount of unjustified Cartesian/Newtonian assumptions which might implicitly “block” alternative ideas). With respect to scientific experimentation (and data collection) Eddington provided the following brilliant analogy in his book “The Philosophy of Physical Science” (1938) which illustrates how the “conceptual/perceptual cognitive net” determines the character of scientific inquiry:
“Let us suppose that an ichthyologist is exploring the life of the ocean. He casts a net into the water and brings up a fishy assortment. Surveying his catch, he proceeds in the usual manner of a scientist to systematise what it reveals. He arrives at two generalisations:
1) No sea-creature is less than two inches long.
2) All sea-creatures have gills.
These are both true of his catch, and he assumes tentatively that they will remain true however often he repeats it. In applying this analogy, the catch stands for the body of knowledge which constitutes physical science, and the net for the sensory and intellectual equipment which we use in obtaining it. The casting of the net corresponds to observation; for knowledge which has not been or could not be obtained by observation is not admitted into physical science. An onlooker may object that the first generalisation is wrong. ‘There are plenty of sea-creatures under two inches long, only your net is not adapted to catch them.’ The icthyologist dismisses this objection contemptuously. ‘Anything uncatchable by my net is ipso facto outside the scope of icthyological knowledge. In short, what my net can’t catch isn’t fish.’ Or — to translate the analogy — ‘If you are not simply guessing, you are claiming a knowledge of the physical universe discovered in some other way than by the methods of physical science, and admittedly unverifiable by such methods. You are a metaphysician. Bah!’”

We submit that 5-MeO-DMT is by far the most effective pharmacological agent for the controlled induction of nondual states of consciousness and consequently the restructuring of the “cognitive net”. Given the postulated complementarity between psyche & physis, nondual states of consciousness are accompanied by physiological changes in the neuronal architecture of the brain, i.e., the conceptual net (mind) and the neuronal net (matter) are complementary aspect of the same underlying “substance”, a tertium quid, viz., universal consciousness. In simplistic terms, by changing the brain the mind changes and, vice versa, by changing the mind the brain changes. Consciousness itself remains unaffected. Therefore, it does not make much sense to speak about “states of consciousness”. Consciousness is an immutable singular — it is the experiencer (that which experiences change — viz., “it” is the nontransient observer of the fluctuating contents of the mind).
5-MeO-DMT has been described as a prototypical entheogen (Metzner, 2015) and it is psychologically and pharmacologically much more potent than its structural relatives (e.g., N,N-Dimethyltryptamine), i.e., qualitatively and quantitatively. An entheogen (Ruck, Bigwood, Staples, Ott, & Wasson, 1979) is a chemical substance (used in a religious, shamanic, or spiritual contexts) that has the potential to produce profound psycho-spiritual insights and changes. From a philological point of view, the etymology of the neologism “entheogen” is a compound lexeme derived from the ancient Greek ἔνθεος (entheos) and γενέσθαι (genesthai) and translates into “generating the divine from within“ (cf. “enthusiasm”). 5-MeO-DMT is a ceremonial sacrament (eucharist) of the “Church of the Tree of Life”. Hence, interdisciplinary research on 5-MeO-DMT might provide further impetus for the emerging new neuroscientific paradigm which goes by the name “neurotheology” (Winkelman, 2004). Following this line of thought it has been stated by the eminent neurobiologist Efrain C. Azmitia that “the ability of these drugs to induce a feeling of closeness to God is a special property of the indoles and this property is attributed to activation of the cortical 2A serotonin receptor” (Azmitia, 2012).
We would like to recapitulate the crux of our argument: Given its phenomenological profundity and its unparalleled efficiency to dissolve ego structures we propose that the psychological effects of 5-MeO-DMT and associated neural correlates should be systematically investigated in order to elucidate the postulated connection between nondual (ego-less) states of consciousness and the stipulated enhancement of creativity. One pillar of this hypothesis is the idea that ego-dissolution is associated with a breakdown of linguistic structures41F43F43F (hence the characteristic ineffability of its phenomenology/quale). According to the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis of linguistic relativism (Sapir, 1929), language structures cognition and perception in significant ways. Ergo, we hypothesize that a release from the strong aprioristic schematising influences of linguistic processes facilitates a more unrestrained (pre-linguistic) style of cognition and perception. Further, we argue that the collapse of the psychophysical “subject versus object” dichotomy into nondual experience has enormous potential for complex cognitive restructuring at multiple levels of analysis (cf. Josipovic, 2010). “Ego exitus” (the apex of ego-dissolution, i.e., ego death) is emotionally and cognitively extremely challenging, an observation which resonates with the “hardship model of creativity” (Forgeard, 2013). At the same time the extremely challenging experience of ego-dissolution and ultimately ego-death may have significant positive therapeutic/cathartic effects which are of essential pertinence in in relation to creativity research (e.g., release from severe traumata, access to repressed unconscious materials, surfacing of archetypal symbolisms, etc.). The experiences induced by 5-MeO-DMT are tremendously radical42F44F44F and therefore capable to disperse deeply engrained cognitive/perceptual schemata43F45F45F , thereby enabling a more unrestricted style of cognition.4F4 Specifically, we argue that due to its unparalleled ego-dissolving properties 5-MeO-DMT facilitates a less self-centred and hence more unbiased style of cognition which is a condicio sine qua non for creativity. This hypothesis is empirically falsifiable in the Popperian sense and various established cognitive testing procedures45F47F47F could be utilized to test this hypothesis experimentally. (Creativity is generally conceptualised as a multi-dimensional construct and various facets of creativity may be affected differentially.) The logic which undergirds our theorizing can be formalized using propositional logic, i.e., in form of a syllogistic argument (see Syllogism #1 below). Specifically, we postulate that ego-dissolution can culminate in a state of nondual consciousness which is phenomenologically, biochemically, and neuroanatomically similar to those reported in numerous ancient wisdom traditions such as Advaita Vedānta and Mahāyāna Buddhism. Nondual experiences have been reported since time immemorial and, for the first time, science is now in a position to investigate them in a systematic fashion (ideally in a holistic manner across multiple levels of explanation, i.e., by combining the epistemic/qualitative with the ontic/quantitative level of analysis). It is hypothesized that nondual states of consciousness induced by 5-MeO-DMT shift the global connectivity patterns between intrinsic networks and extrinsic networks. These networks are thought to be anticorrelated. In terms of large-scale neuroanatomical organization. According to current knowledge, the intrinsic network includes rather broadly defined areas in the medial prefrontal cortices, the posterior cingulate cortices, the precunei, the inferior parietal lobule, and temporal areas such as parts of the hippocampi (Cavanna, 2007; Josipovic, 2014). Its activity has been associated with a broad array of self-referential cognitions such as self-awareness and self-reflection, executive functions like future planning, and also creativity. For instance, it has been experimentally demonstrated that distraction (i.c., mind-wandering) can enhance creativity (B. Baird et al., 2012). We argue, in abstracto, that “a release from the limiting self” is an important aspect in the context of creativity enhancement (to put it metaphorically, the unleashing of creativity from the grip of the ego). The general idea is that signal transduction in neuroanatomical areas associated with self-referential cognition is reduced. For instance, it has been demonstrated that meditation is associated with decreased activity in the default mode network, i.c., decreased activity in medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortices (Brewer et al., 2011). We expect similar (but more pronounced) effects with 5-MeO-DMT, specifically given the phenomenological similarities between 5-MeO-DMT and meditative (nondual) states of consciousness. Based on previous conceptually related work which investigated the neural correlates of nondual states in meditators it seems indicated to examine if the neuronal activity changes induced by 5-MeO-DMT are congruent with ROIs related to nondual states of consciousness achieved via mediation, e.g., the involvement of the DMN and specifically the central precuneus (cf. Josipovic, 2014). This line of research is particularly warranted given the plethora of studies which correlate meditation with creativity (Baas, Nevicka, & Ten Velden, 2014; Capurso, Fabbro, & Crescentini, 2014; Colzato, Ozturk, & Hommel, 2012; Dawson, 2004; Ding, Tang, Tang, & Posner, 2014; Domino, 1976; Horan, 2009; B. C. N. Müller, Gerasimova, & Ritter, 2016; Ostafin & Kassman, 2012; Peet, 1979). Our primary argument can be stated in form of a logically valid Aristotelian categorical syllogism.

Syllogism #1
Major premise:
Minor premise:
Deductive conclusion: Ego-dissolution enhances creativity.
5-MeO-DMT induces ego-dissolution.
∴ Ergo, 5-MeO-DMT enhances creativity.

Syllogism #2
Major premise:
Minor premise:
Deductive conclusion: Increases in FCD are predictive of creativity.
5-MeO-DMT increases global functional connectivity density.
∴ Ergo, 5-MeO-DMT enhances creativity.

Syllogism #3
Major premise:
Minor premise:
Deductive conclusion: Downregulation of the DMN enhances creativity.
5-MeO-DMT downregulates the DMN.
∴ Ergo, 5-MeO-DMT enhances creativity.

According to syllogistic logic each of the three distinct terms represents a category, i.c.:
[Ego-dissolution] — [5-MeO-DMT] — [Creativity].
For example, in Syllogism #1 the category [Creativity] is the major term and [5-MeO-DMT] constitutes the minor term. Crucially, the premises have a single term in common (the middle term) which appears as the subject or predicate of the categorical proposition, in casu, [Ego-dissolution]. According to the principles of propositional logic, the conclusion follows deductively iff the major and minor premise are accepted as veridical. Based on these syllogistic arguments we formulate the ensuing falsifiable a priori hypotheses:

H1: Downregulation of the default-mode network (DMN) predicts subsequent increases in creativity (see below for details on the incubation/integration-phase and the “DMN rebound effect”). In addition, we conjecture that 5-MeO-DMT decreases depression similar to results obtained with psilocybin and we predict a correlation between reduction in depressive symptoms and isochronous increases in creativity.
H2: The self-reported intensity of 5-MeO-DMT phenomenology (post eventum introspective assessment) predicts subsequent increases in global functional connectivity density which are in turn predictive of creativity (the differential effects on various facets of creativity should be investigated — we suggest that “mathematical creativity” is a specifically interesting aspect given the highly geometrical characteristics of DMT phenomenology). Further, we predict that 5-MeO-DMT induces neuro/synaptoplastic changes which are crucial in the context of creativity and cognitive/neuronal restructuring (cf. Dakic, 2017). In addition, we predict based on prior research that 5-MEO-DMT induces neurogenesis via various neurotrophic growth factors, e.g., hippocampal neurogenesis (Catlow et al., 2013). Dendritic complexification and synaptic sprouting may have a psychological analogon.
H3: Self-reported ego-dissolution phenomenology predicts subsequent enhancements in creativity, as quantified by various creativity test batteries (e.g., J. C. Kaufman, 2012) in a dose-dependent manner. This effect is mediated by the profundity of the experience, e.g., how challenging the experience was, intensity of the “peak experience”, personal meaningfulness of the experience, etc. (cf. Barrett, Bradstreet, Leoutsakos, Johnson, & Griffiths, 2016; Forgeard, 2013; Griffiths et al., 2006; Majić, Schmidt, & Gallinat, 2015).
H4: The intensity of 5-MeO-DMT induced ego-dissolution predicts longitudinal increases in aesthetic perception, biophilia, and feelings of fundamental existential interconnectedness46F48F48F (viz., OTE – similar to studies which focused on the structural analogue psilocybin (MacLean et al., 2011), but more pronounced; a direct comparison between compounds would be of interest to infer structure-activity relationships based on neuroimaging-data and qualitative phenomenological differences).
H5: The intensity of ego-dissolution predicts the longitudinally measured significance of the life-event in a non-linear dose-dependent manner, similar to the patterns observed in studies with the psilocybin (Griffiths, Richards, Johnson, McCann, & Jesse, 2008).
H6: 5-MeO-DMT modulates activity in the limbic system (i.e., amygdala, insular cortex) in a longitudinal manner (cf. Kraehenmann et al., 2015). Various biomarkers (GSR, plasma glucocorticoid-levels, etc.) could be quantified to test this prediction. This hypothesis is not only interesting in relation to interplay between stress and creativity (Byron, Khazanchi, & Nazarian, 2010) but also for the treatment of anxiety disorders such as PTSD.
H7: Given that 5-MeO-DMT can have strong somatosensory and viscerosensory effects (somasthæsia, changes in proprioception and body-image, and in some cases out-of-body experiences) we hypothesize that various somatosensitive areas are involved, specifically the temporo-parietal junction (potentially also the anterior insular cortex (Yu et al., 2018)). This hypothesis is particularly intriguing from an embodied cognition perspective on creativity and also in relation to Damasio’s somatic marker hypothesis.
H8: The peak-experience of ego-death (sometimes colloquially referred as “break-through” or in the extensive Indian literature Nirvikalpa Samādhi49F49F ) is marked by a rapid phase-shift in global neuronal activity (e.g., quantifiable via simultaneous EEG/fMRI). We predict a marked increase in creativity in comparison with states of consciousness which still include rudimentary traces of the self-concept. We term this mnemonically the “less ego => more creativity hypothesis”. We conjecture that this transformative nondual peak-experience is accompanied by marked epigenetic changes (cf. Dias & Ressler, 2014). Further, we predict that 5-MeO-DMT effects genetic health on multiple levels (e.g., telomeres and telomerase activity) and that overall genetic health is associated with creativity. This hypothesis is based on studies which demonstrated positive correlations between mediation, mental health, and genetics (Alda et al., 2016; Epel, Daubenmier, Moskowitz, Folkman, & Blackburn, 2009).
H9: We predict that 5-MeO-DMT effects social cognition in numerous ways. The “realization of unity” fosters prosocial attitudes, empathy, altruism/reciprocity. This hypothesis can be tested using standard procedures from social psychology and we predict interactions of 5-MeO-DMT with the oxytocin/vasopressin neuropeptide systems. The link between empathy and creativity has been noted in prior research (Carlozzi, Bull, Eells, & Hurlburt, 1995). Further, we suggest that terror-management-theory is an interesting explanatory framework with respect to ego-death and creativity (cf. Arndt & Vess, 2008; Routledge & Arndt, 2009).
H10: Finally, we predict that 5-MeO-DMT synergizes with other therapeutic modalities in a non-linear fashion, i.e., the effectiveness of other therapies can be enhanced by 5-MeO-DMT because it creates a state of psychological receptiveness in which defence mechanisms are curtailed. We specifically predict that 5-MeO-DMT has strong longitudinal synergistic effects when combined with mediation (particularly types of mediation that foster nondual philosophical contemplations). This hypothesis is partly motivated by recent neuroimaging work which reported quantitative synergistic effects between psilocybin and mindfulness training (Smigielski et al., 2019). The combinatorial effects of mediation and 5-MeO-DMT on creativity are predicted to be significantly “larger than the sum of its parts” (i.e., larger than would be predicted based on a linear additive model).

Ex hypothesi, we argue that the conjectured effects are objectively quantifiable and reliably replicable in rigorously controlled experimental settings. The paucity of research on 5-MeO-DMT is surprising, specifically given that it is an endogenous component of neurobiology which implies an evolutionary function (neurochemical vestigiality is an unlikely explanation). Up to date, we are unaware of any systematic scientific research which focuses specifically on the effects of 5-MeO-DMT on ego-dissolution and creativity. Consequently, we suggest that future studies should be designed in order to elucidate this rich and potentially very fruitful research area. The present discussion is just a start in order to motivate future studies along these lines. Among the various psychoactive tryptamines (e.g. psilocybin/psilocin, DMT/NMT/α,N-DMT, Nω-methylserotonin, convolutindole A, 5-Bromo-DMT, etc.) 5-MeO-DMT is specifically suitable for controlled scientific experimentation due to the short duration of its acute effects. It can be utilised as an inhalant (e.g., vaporization) and its onset it extremely fast (a single inhalation is sufficient for its full effects which manifest instantaneously, often before the exhalation of the vapor). Alternative routes of administration include IV, intracerebroventricular, intramuscular, intranasal, intrarectal, intravaginal, sublingual, or oral administration in combination with a monoamine oxidase inhibitor (MAOI) to prevent enzymatic deamination in the GI tract (Halberstadt, 2016). However, with the latter synergistic method may change pharmacodynamics (and psychoactivity) in hitherto unknows ways and negative side effects such as increased “body-load” have been mentioned in anecdotal reports. Data indicates that 5-MeO-DMT may, under some circumstances, be toxic if administered orally in combination with a MAOI. Specifically, there may be genetic/phenotypic interindividual differences in polymorphic cytochrome P450 2D6 (see H. W. Shen, Wu, Jiang, & Yu, 2010) which is encoded by the CYP2D6 gene. Future studies should address interindividual differences in CYP2D6 and their systematic relation to pharmacokinetics and phenomenology. Given the existence of considerable interindividual variations in the efficiency and amount of CYP2D6 enzyme produced, it is plausible to hypothesize that there are subgroups who metabolize 5-MeO-DMT rapidly while other are “moderate or slow metabolizers”. Furthermore, interethnic differences in genetic polymorphism of CYP2D6 have been indicated (Teh & Bertilsson, 2012) and it would be of interest how these relate to psychological variables. Pharmacogenomic investigations are thus warranted and we specifically suggest that genetic differences should be correlated with neuronal and phenomenological variables. For instance, slow 5-MeO-DMT metabolism should have significant effects on fMRI/EEG signatures and the duration of the experimental time-course. This in turn should correlate significantly with qualitative phenomenology and intensity of the experience, and hence, with the overall psychological impact and consequently changes in creativity (i.e., genetics variability => pharmacokinetics => neural correlates => subjective phenomenology => psychological impact => creativity).
We suggest that the effects of 5-MeO-DMT on creativity (and cognition in general) should be investigated in a longitudinal design. Controlled experimentation should be interdisciplinary and multi-modal (i.e., brain imaging47F50F50F , self-reports, introspective measures, etc.) in order to gain a complete picture of the effects of the compound (i.e., methodological triangulation/complementary measurement methods). For instance, brain-wide network dynamics which undergird creativity are currently a cutting-edge neuroscience topic of great interest (Beaty, Benedek, Silvia, & Schacter, 2016; Beaty et al., 2018) and functional connectivity density mapping (FCDM) is widely utilised (Tomasi & Volkow, 2010). The general idea is that creative thought involves dynamic interactions between large-scale brain systems, specifically between cortical hubs within the default mode network, the salience network, and the prefrontal executive systems. Note that this is a somewhat “fuzzy” neuroanatomical analysis as each of the networks includes a vast number of anatomical structures. Our hypothesis contributes a more precise predictions and we argue that specifically the downregulation of the default-mode network (which, according to theory, is accompanied by the phenomenology of ego-dissolution) is an important component for the enhancement of creativity. Further, we predict a priori that the downregulation of DMN activity creates a rebound-effect (based on the Aristotelian principle of homeostatic self-regulation, which is incorporated into numerous contemporary complex systems/cybernetics theories).48F51F51F Hence, pertaining to brain dynamics and FCD the time-course should be taken into account in order “serialize” creative idea formation. The “incubation and integration phase” (after the acute/proximal neuropharmacological effects subside) is of particular interest with regard to neuroplasticity and creativity (i.e., statistical time series analysis should be employed to map various parameters diachronically). Given the profundity of the experience occasioned by 5-MeO-DMT the integration-phase can span several weeks and even months (some would even verify that it lasts a human lifetime).
Preliminary (quasi-experimental) research suggests that structurally related tryptamines can foster pro-environmental “nature-relatedness” and (anti-neoliberal) egalitarian attitudes (Forstmann & Sagioglou, 2017; Lyons & Carhart-Harris, 2018; Nour, Evans, & Carhart-Harris, 2017). Controlled experimental research along these lines is of great importance in the current survival-threatening economic/socio-political climate (see also Sugarman, 2015).
Hypothesis 8 (H8) entails a prediction pertaining to potential epigenetic changes induced by 5-MeO-DMT. This ideas is derived from recent genetic studies which reintroduced Lamarckian elements into quantitative biology and thereby challenge the “central dogma of molecular biology” (Crick, 1970) which was for a long time unquestionably axiomatic to genetic research. For instance, it has been shown that acquired olfactory conditioning can be epigenetically inherited by subsequent generations (at least up to F2) (Dias & Ressler, 2014). The odorant receptor (Olfr151) was used to condition F0 mice and subsequent generations (which were utterly naïve to the olfactory conditioning paradigm) revealed CpG hypomethylation in the Olfr151 gene. We argue that if simple olfactory conditioning can cause quantifiable quasi-Lamarckian epigenetic effects than a profound 5-MeO-DMT experience should be equally quantifiable at the genetic level (cf. Heard & Martienssen, 2014; Trerotola, Relli, Simeone, & Alberti, 2015). We suggest that genes associated with the 5-HT (serotonin) system (e.g., SLC6A4 gene associated with sodium-dependent serotonin transporter) are a likely genetic locus for a priori (planned) comparisons (specifically in relation to depression and anxiety; cf. hypothesis H1 & H6). For instance, it has been reported that individuals with specific serotonin transporter (5-HTT) promoter polymorphism (associated with reduced 5-HTT expression) exhibit greater amygdala activation (fear and anxiety-related behaviours) as assessed by BOLD functional magnetic resonance imaging (Hariri, 2002; see also Heinz et al., 2005). Interestingly, it has been experimentally demonstrated that psilocybin decreases amygdala reactivity and that this limbic downregulation correlates with enhanced positive mood (Kraehenmann et al., 2015). Ergo, we predict similar effects for 5-MeO-DMT.

N.B.: We close this section with a cautionary note. Despite the centrality and prominence of neuroscientific research in contemporary popular discourse (cf. representativeness heuristic) our current knowledge about the brain is very limited (neuroscience is still in its infantile developmental phase) and therefore any intervention (be it electrical, chemical, or otherwise) into this highly complex system should be considered very carefully and thoroughly. Deliberate ethical considerations are of utmost importance (intellectual humility is a genuine scientific virtue in this respect (see Gregg, Mahadevan, & Sedikides, 2017)). The rather “dark” history of neuroscience (unfortunately) demonstrates an absolute lack of caution and ethical conscience (e.g., frontal lobotomy/leucotomy, electroconvulsive “shock therapy”, destructive psychopharmacological interventions in children, etc. pp.). Some of these ethically highly questionable “treatments” are still utilized today (cf. U. J. Müller et al., 2013; Schläpfer & Kayser, 2014). For an excellent discussion of neuroethics and the amoral history of psychiatry and neurosurgery see (Breggin, 1998, 2008). For a critique of psychopharmacology with reference to human values and various “rational principles of psychopharmacology” see (Breggin, 2003, 2016) .
We submit that endogenous compounds such as 5-MeO-DMT (with a long evolutionary history of human usage) are a casus specialis. Note that 5-MeO-DMT has been associated with neurorestorative adaptations (Dakic, 2017) whereas widely prescribed psychopharmaceuticals such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs, e.g., fluoxetine/Prozac®) and norepinephrine–dopamine reuptake inhibitors (NDRIs, e.g., methylphenidate/Ritalin®) are known to cause chronic brain impairment (CBI) and various psychological disorders (Breggin, 2008, 2011; Breggin & Breggin, 1996). Consequently, 5-MeO-DMT should not be grouped together with novel/synthetic psychopharmacological substances which are oftentimes completely alien to human neurobiology (an evolutionary/pharmacokinetic argument could be formulated). However, besides “side-effects” such as neurotoxicity there are other significant complications pertaining to powerful psychedelic agents which can produce dramatic changes in consciousness. For instance, how can anyone give “informed consensus” to an experience which is ineffable (cf. the ineffability of quale). Moreover, 5-MeO-DMT can induce the phenomenon of timelessness, or to use William Blakes poetic expression: “Eternity in an hour”.52F52F Superficial descriptions such as “the experiment lasts one hour” thus become meaningless because the compound interact with consciousness per se (cf. Kant on space and time as a priori mental constructs).53F53F Thus, it should be explicitly emphasized that the human brain and consciousness are still largely terra incognita49F (we do not even understand their relation; cf. “the explanatory gap”) and consequently a high degree of caution, foresight, and genuine ethical consideration are advisable with respect to any intervention which might interfere with sensitive and fine-tuned biological processes.

Brains in chains: Neuropolitics, neurodiversity, and cognitive liberty
In January 2016, the “Psychoactive Substances Act” (PSA) reached Royal Assent in the United Kingdom.50F54F54F The PSA generically prohibits all mind-altering substances besides the most harmful and addictive ones which are of commercial significance (e.g., alcohol and tobacco were explicitly exempted; but see Nutt, King, & Phillips, 2010). This novel legal framework classifies relatively harmless substances like psilocybin on par with the most harmful and detrimental substances such as heroin and cocaine. The UK thus became the first country in human history which generically banned all psychoactive substances, viz., a juridical omnibus prohibition of all mind-altering chemicals was ratified, irrespective of their well-documented historicity and their comparative safety profile (King, 2013), for example, as objectively quantified by the conventional LD50 and TD50 toxicity indices. For instance, psilocybin is non-addictive (in fact it has been effectively utilised for addiction treatment (Bogenschutz & Forcehimes, 2017)) and it exhibits remarkably low toxicity. The in vivo LD50 in humans remains unknown due to the paucity of any intentional or accidental poisoning death data.51F55F55F The therapeutic window (also known as “pharmaceutical window”) for psilocybin is comparatively very safe (Gable, 1993) and the maximum tolerated dose (MTD) is very high (Zhuk et al., 2015). Further, the therapeutic index (TI) is very high (Rucker, 2015). The TI quantifies the toxic dose as a ratio of the effective dose: TI=(¬¬¬〖TD〗_50 )/〖ED〗_50 . From a toxicological point of view a higher TI is thus preferable to a lower one.
Rank-ordered therapeutic indices for various psychoactive substances:
Heroin ≈ 6
Alcohol ≈ 10
Cocaine ≈ 15
Psilocybin ≈ 1000
Based on this hierarchical collocation it has been argued that “psychedelic drugs should be legally reclassified so that researchers can investigate their therapeutic potential” (this is indeed the title of the article by Rucker, 2015). A common metric in comparative risk assessment is the margin of exposure (MOE), defined as the ratio between the toxicological threshold (defined as the benchmark dose) and the estimated average human intake. Both, MTD and MOE indicate a very benign safety profile for psilocybin, especially compared to widely used neurotoxic agents like alcohol which, per contra, has a very low MOE (Lachenmeier & Rehm, 2015) and has been associated with numerous detrimental neurocognitive (Weitemier & Ryabinin, 2003), genetic, and epigenetic effects (Y. Chen, Ozturk, & Zhou, 2013). Despite these scientific facts, psilocybin is classified as a “Class A substance” in the UK. The PSA 2016 can be regarded as a lex specialis which introduces serious burdens (viz., judicial onus) on researchers interested in neurobiology and consciousness. Ergo, scientific research on psychedelics is currently legally highly restricted due to the irrational Class A status of psychedelic substances (despite numerous “privileged” exceptions). The classification of psilocybin is de jure based on the fallacious presupposition that psilocybin has “no medicinal value” — a conjecture which is de facto clearly not veridical as psilocybin has numerous medical applications (but see Bogenschutz & Johnson, 2016). Therefore, the legal classification is, a fortiori, inadequate. The case of 5-MeO-DMT is specifically paradoxical given that it is a natural endogenous component of the neurochemical composition of the human brain. Furthermore, the PSA has obvious implications for the perception of psychedelics in the public sphere. Contrary to widespread public doxa (Bourdieu, 1977), epidemiological data indicate that psychedelics are not linked to psychopathology or suicidal behaviour (Johansen & Krebs, 2015; Krebs & Johansen, 2013; cf. K. Müller, Püschel, & Iwersen-Bergmann, 2013) as purported by numerous coordinated transnational large-scale mass-media campaigns (starting in the 1960s) which utilized propagandistic/PR methods à la Bernays (Bernays, 1928; L’Etang, 1999) in order justify the governmental “War on Drugs” (initiated by the Nixon administration) which was clearly politically motivated, for instance, in order to target Vietnam War opponents and racial minorities, and to serve the “prison-industrial complex” (Douglas & Pond, 2012; L. D. Moore & Elkavich, 2008). Given the extensive initial media-coverage of the campaign, the public mind is till this day still under the influence of this intentional misinformation (unconscious/implicit associations play an important rôle in social attitudes). It would require enormous orchestrated long-term efforts to counterbalance these negative stereotypical attitudes (pro bono publico).
Well informed legal scholars interpret the PSA as an explicit violation of the fundamental right to mental self-determination (i.e., cognitive liberty; Walsh, 2016) – particularly with respect to Article 9 of the European Convention on Human Rights 1988 (§1-2) which should protect the right to freedom of thought. It is obvious that cognitive liberty is a prerequisite for creativity. The PSA reduces neurodiversity and it juridically justifies the homogenisation of cognitive/neuronal processes. It selectively restricts cultural and memetic diversity and, consequently, cultural and cognitive evolution (per analogiam with the crucial importance of genetic diversity for biological evolution). The loss of memetic and cultural diversity (i.e., variability in Weltanschauungen) is a serious problem. From an anthropological vantage point, memetic homogenisation (i.e., the rapidly accelerating trend towards cultural and psychological monomorphisms due to Western hegemony) is a global and rapidly accelerating trend (this “Eurocentric phasing” is also reflected in a global reduction of linguistic diversity, inter alia). Cultural and memetic diversity are as important to the “human superorganism” as genetic diversity is for adaptivity of immune-systems (a scale-independent quasi-Darwinian argument which emphasises the general importance of diversity for evolution at the micro and the macro level of various complex systems). In sensu lato, the loss of cultural and memetic diversity reduces the psychological and hence behavioural capacity of the human species to react to novel (unpredictable) challenges. In a similar manner immunodiversity (e.g., antibody diversity) is crucial in order to react to novel immunological challenges, e.g., de novo mutations in the genome of viral and bacterial pathogens, etc. A reduction of diversity ipso facto implies a reduction in degrees of freedom (e.g., possibilities to respond to a given stressor) and hence robustness and mobility, due to a reduction in combinatorial possibilities. “Conformational flexibility” (Rizzo, Tinello, Pearlstein, & Taniuchi, 1999) can thus be conceptually compared to “cognitive flexibility” (Scott, 2006), and perhaps similar dynamical/topological models can be applied. In short, cultural and memetic diversity are as important to human societal systems as genetic diversity is to biological systems.
Summa summarum, the PSA has far-reaching ramifications. It is de facto not evidence-based and presents a serious legal impediment to scientific research, creativity, memetic and cultural diversity, cognitive innovation, and cognitive liberty (see also Boire, 2000).

Potential for military abuse: Neuroethics and the “ticking bomb scenario”
Given the fact that 5-MeO-DMT is unparallel in its ability to virtually instantaneously dissolve psychological ego structures (viz., ego dissolution par excellence) it can in principle be utilized as a “neuropsychological weapon”, for instance, in the context of military operations. Like every powerful scientific tool, it is a two-sided sword (a neurochemical Janus) that can be used to elevate and unfold human potential, creativity, and consciousness or, vice versa, to manipulate, control, and suppress it. In the past, methods that are capable of dissolving ego structures have been of great interest to the military in relation to “enhanced interrogation” — a euphemism for torture (see also O’Mara, 2009). For example, the collaboration of Donald Hebb with the CIA and various military agencies which focused on sensory deprivation/overload and other techniques to “dismantle the self” (Brown, 2007; McCoy, 2006, 2007). Many of the resulting procedures can be found in the only recently declassified U.S. Army and CIA interrogation manuals (known as the KUBARK “torture manuals”).54F58F58F For various reasons the military has vested interests in research on neurochemistry and creativity and the “weaponization of neuroscience” is a general problem with extensive ethical ramifications which should be much more prominently discussed under the generic header of “neuroethics” (e.g., Farah, 2005). In sensu lato, creativity is a prerequisite for scientific innovation. On their official website DARPA explicitly articulates that their “success depends on the vibrant ecosystem of innovation within which the agency operates”. It is important to keep in mind that DARPAs enunciated mission is “to create revolutions in military science and to maintain technological dominance over the rest of the world […] with an annual budget of roughly $3 billion” and that “DARPA as an agency does not conduct scientific research. Its [on average 120] program managers and directors hire defense contractors, academics, and other government organizations to do the work” (Jacobsen, 2015; content in bracket added). A related agency which was founded in 2006 is the IARPA (Intelligence Advanced Research Projects Activity) which describes its mission as follows: “To envision and lead high-risk, high-payoff research that delivers innovative technology for future overwhelming intelligence advantage.” IARPA provides significant funding for academia and industry research across a broad array of areas including neuroscience, cognitive psychology, chemistry, biology, mathematics, physics, computer science, linguistics, and political science, inter alia. IARPAs “moon-shot” programs are intended to enable researchers to engage in “ideas that are potentially disruptive to the status quo”. Other research areas are “forecasting tournaments” (Tetlock, Mellers, Rohrbaugh, & Chen, 2014) and “hybrid forecasting competitions” which aims to “improve accuracy in predicting worldwide geopolitical issues, including foreign political elections, interstate conflict, disease outbreaks, and economic indicators by leveraging the relative strengths of humans and machines” (see also Weinberger, 2011). Moreover, artificial intelligence is a highly prioritised domain which is actively pursuit by DARPA & IARPA. The book entitled “Mind Wars: Brain Science and the Military in the 21st Century” by Moreno (2012) provides a broader picture on the tight interconnections between science, academia, and the military-industrial complex, specifically with respect to “mind control”. Moreno submits the following “hypothetical” scenario which highlights the importance of creativity research from a military perspective.

“Here’s a science fiction scenario: an army of robots capable of movement nearly as precise as that of a human soldier, each controlled by an individual hundreds or even thousands of miles away. These automata could undertake actions that would be foolhardy for human beings but worth the tactical risk for machines; because they are controlled by people, they would have the benefit of creativity that might limit even the most advanced android. But the old-fashioned remote control scenario would have the operator pushing buttons or moving levers while seeing on a monitor what the robot is seeing, a method that would be far too clumsy for the instantaneous reactions often required in combat. What is wanted is a technology that would allow the robot to respond as soon as the distant operator does. Such a technology would, in effect, have to be able to read the intentions of the operator, his or her thoughts themselves, not merely respond to the operator’s muscle movements through a mechanical apparatus.” (Moreno, 2012, p.39).

Human creatures have the unique capacity for creativity which even the most complex automata hitherto lack. Creativity is thus a topic of great interest with regard to brain-computer interfaces (Vaadia, 2009) and a detailed understanding of the cognitive, neurochemical, and neuromechanical basis of creativity is of fundamental interest to AI research which is currently utterly unable to emulate genuine creativity in silico (cf. Boden, 2014), a problem which, we argue, is intimately related to the “hard problem of consciousness” (Chalmers, 1995), i.e., the unsolved question of how quantitative processes such as electrochemical neuronal signal transduction relate to consciousness (and hence creativity). This “explanatory gap” (Block & Stalnaker, 1999), which is based on a dualistic Cartesian mind-matter conceptualisation, is a serious obstacle for reductive materialism (Levine, 1983) and consequently military research on robotics and AI. This research domain is currently heavily funded by various sectors of industry. Particularly autonomous weapon systems are a very active area of research (but see Bohannon, 2015). “Autonomous creative systems” (Saunders, 2012), “creative cognitive computation” (d’Inverno & Luck, 2012) and “creative evolutionary systems” (Bentley & Corne, 2002) are at the cutting-edge of contemporary transdisciplinary AI research.
Closely related to computational applications, creativity is also a crucial asset with respect to game theory (e.g., enhancing one’s own creativity while reducing the opponent’s capacity for creativity is an effective stratagem). For example, artificial swarm intelligence systems utilise game-theoretical calculi which require “computational creativity” for the self-organisation of networked multi-agent systems (al-Rifaie, Bishop, & Caines, 2012) and it could be argued that creativity determines the “degrees of freedom” of a given system. In sum, autonomous systems are most effective when they have the capacity to execute creative manoeuvres (predictability and cognitive/computational inflexibility are an operational and strategic disadvantage). With history in sight, the question how these evolving “creative” autonomous systems are utilised in the future is a topic of great humanistic concern, specifically given the concentration of financial and hence political power alluded to in the introduction of this paper (see also Lin, Bekey, & Abney, 2008). It needs to be accentuated that surveillance of the general civil populace is an ongoing issue (Bauman et al., 2014) and that we are currently observing a restriction of freedom of speech (e.g., algorithmic censorship) in many domains (Arquilla, 2011; Mausfeld, 2017, 2019). We argue that the developments of creative military AI have to be evaluated against this broader historical and societal background as these systems, once installed and implemented, could be utilized for plutocratic/totalitarian cybernetic social control (and not just the ostensible “war” on alleged terrorists which might indeed be a deceptive pretext — a fairly creative and demonstrably effective “foot-in-the-door” technique, as social psychologists call it (Freedman & Fraser, 2017)).
With respect to “deceptive pretexts” another obviously related area of vested military interest is research on lying and the detection of lies. Numerous studies have demonstrated that creativity is related to the ability to deceive and to conceal information (Gino & Ariely, 2012; Gino & Wiltermuth, 2014; H. Kapoor & Khan, 2017) which is pivotal in relation to military operations as the military routinely makes use of deceptive manoeuvres, as for example evidenced by operation NORTHWOODS. That is, the ability to lie is linked to various facets of creativity such as divergent thinking (Walczyk, Runco, Tripp, & Smith, 2008). Likewise the ability to detect deception (e.g., deception detection accuracy) appears to be correlated with creativity measures (cf. Walczyk & Griffith-Ross, 2008). Hence, creativity is pivotal with regard to military interrogation tactics (which frequently include torture).
A historical vantage point is crucial to frame the discussion appropriately. Psychopharmaceutical interventions were a decisive component of the German Blitzkrieg in WWII, for instance, the utilisation of methamphetamine (Heal, Smith, Gosden, & Nutt, 2013) as an analeptic cognitive and physical performance booster. During the 3rd Reich, Nazi “scientists” conducted extremely inhuman medical and psychological experiments in concentration camps (even on children). These experiments also involved psychedelics (and other neuroactive chemicals) for the purpose of interrogation and psychological control, e.g., mescaline experiment in concentration camps in Dachau and Auschwitz. Mescaline was the first psychedelic known to Western science and it was used in the brutal “aviation tests” at Dachau in which prisoners were crushed and frozen to death (Jay, 2019). The specified objective was “to eliminate the will of the person examined” with the overarching goal to develop a “truth serum” (see also Keller, 2004). The mescaline experiments were conducted by Dr. Kurt Plötner (University of Leipzig). After the war more than 1700 high-ranking German scientists (some of whom committed the most appalling crimes against humanity) emigrated to the USA via the top-secret operation PAPERCLIP (so named because of the tons of “paperclipped” German documents which were brought to the USA). Eventually, these scientists worked jointly in classified programs with code-names such as CHATTER, BLUEBIRD, ARTICHOKE, and MK-ULTRA (but see Jacobsen, 2014).59F59F Dr. Plötner was amongst them and joined the BLUEBIRD task force. A review of Jacobson’s book (op. cit.) which is published on the official CIA website (see URL in footnote)55F60F60F provides the following synopsis:

“In 1949, the CIA created the Office of Scientific Intelligence. Its first director, Dr. Willard Machle, traveled to Germany to set up a special program to interrogate Soviet spies. The CIA believed the Russians had developed mind-control programs and wanted to know how US spies would hold up against this capability if caught. He also aimed to explore the feasibility of creating a ‘Manchurian candidate’ through behavioral modification. Thus, Operation Bluebird was born. Bluebird, later called MKULTRA, was a research activity experimenting in behavioral engineering of humans. The Nuremberg Code prohibits experimentation with humans without their consent. During this program, Dr. Frank Olson, a US Army biological weapons researcher, was given the drug LSD without his knowledge, leading to his death by leaping from a building. DCI Richard Helms ordered much of the documentation destroyed, and the circumstances of his demise remain controversial to this day.”

Given that a large proportion of documents was intentionally destroyed the full scope and the scientific results of the MK-ULTRA programmes remain unknown. Furthermore, central figures were granted legal immunity and numerous leading scientists simply continued their prestigious careers in psychology, psychiatry, and neuroscience — oftentimes in highly influential positions within mainstream academia. The public never received an adequate explanation why these illegal programs where conducted (besides the usual exorbitantly exaggerated enemy image of Russia). It is likely that the published information only reveals the “tip of the iceberg” and one can only speculate about how far MK-ULTRA really went. It has been stated by Dr. Robert Lashbrook (deputy director of MK-ULTRA) that the available documents were “boiler plate” cover files. However, even the superficial cover stories are clearly extremely alarming from a human rights perspective.
Initially, psychedelics were intended for a new type of warfare. For instance, Dr. Wilson Greene was a proponent of “psychochemical warfare”, i.e., “hallucinogenic or psychotomimetic drugs […] whose effects mimic insanity or psychosis.” Greene argued that if these substances would be used against enemy soldiers that “there can be no doubt that their will to resist would be weakened greatly, if not entirely destroyed, by the mass hysteria and panic which would ensue.” (Jacobsen, 2014). Later, the CIA foresaw much more extensive applications than just psychochemical warfare on the battlefield and it extended these initial programs into new domains (e.g., for the purpose of interrogation and to destabilise individuals and certain “uncongenial” groups within society — such as anti-war political activists). For instance, MK-ULTRA subproject #40 focused on “LSD-type compounds both in laboratory and human beings” and the application of aerosols (“nebulizing nonaqueous solvents”) to deliver various psychochemicals of interest to “non-psychotic humans”. One can only speculate about real-world application methods of such creative ideas.
Psychedelics became a crucial component of the CIA MK-ULTRA “mind control” agenda (which consisted of more than 140 known subprojects, involved more than 80 universities and governmental institutions, and remained completely unnoticed by the public — even congress was allegedly entirely unaware of the multimillion-dollar program). The covert program was outright illegal and utilized LSD-25 and other psychoactive compounds in the most unethical ways possibly imaginable (on unwitting “subjects”). The main purpose of the program was to manipulate and control human beings, both on the individual level and the level of mass psychology. MK-ULTRA weaponised science while the general populace was absolutely nescient about the unethical experiments which were conducted which the aid of taxpayers’ money. In 1975, the “Church Committee” (formally the “United States Senate Select Committee to Study Governmental Operations with Respect to Intelligence Activities”) investigated the voluminous case. The committee’s final report was published in 1976 in six books. The report reveals how clandestine and utterly unethical the governmental agencies operated. Remarkably, one of the topics of focal interest was retrograde amnesia, i.e., how to the erase memory of past events and to then reprogram the mind with new thought and behaviours, e.g., via hypnosis (A. Winter, 2011). Psychedelic played a crucial part in this regard. The permanent changing of personality structures was a related agenda. Techniques involved countless drugs, hypnosis, sleep and sensory deprivation, electroshock experiments, induction of traumata and dissociation, et cetera. Experiments on tens of thousands of people were carried out in the most callous and merciless ways (in universities, hospitals, prisons, military facilities, i.a., without any regard for consensus of the naïve subjects). A large spectrum of human psychology and behaviour was systematically investigated in this large-scale program (Price, 2007). It is noteworthy that one of the main private sector funding bodies was the notorious Rockefeller Foundation.
A disturbing case which has been associated with MK-ULTRA is the legal case referred to as the “Unabomber” (alias Ted Kaczynski). From 1959-62 Harvard psychologist Henry Murray subjected sophomores to psychologically damaging experiments which entailed the administration of LSD-25 and subsequent severe attacks on the personality structures of participants. Evidence indicates that Murrays experimentations were covertly funded via the MK-ULTRA program (Chase, 2000). Interestingly, Murray founded Harvard’s Social Relations Department “which was funded by covert intelligence agencies and the Rockefeller agency through which much of his research was conducted” (Sand, 2007, p. 8). One of the projects was the euphemistically termed “Multiform Assessments of Personality Development Among Gifted College Men” which subjected participants to a series of humiliating interrogations. The covert purpose of this research agenda was to develop an “interrogation-stress test”. The advertised description of the experiment was vague and sounded rather harmless:
“Would you be willing to contribute to the solution of certain psychological problems (parts of an on-going program of research in the development of personality), by serving as a subject in a series of experiments or taking a number of tests (average about 2 hours a week) through the academic year (at the current College rate per hour)?”
Participants where not informed about the severity of the ego-shattering psychological manipulations which would be applied to them (no informed consensus) and the experiments involved psychological methods which are ethically indefensible (such as brutal attacks on the ego under the influence of psychedelics). It is of course difficult to say in how far Kaczynski’s intense hate of psychology and science was influenced by these profoundly traumatic psychedelic experiences and if there is any causal relation between these events and his later crimes which killed numerous people (correlation ≠ causation; i.e., the “cum hoc ergo propter hoc” logical fallacy of implied causality). However, he writes the following in his manifesto “Industrial Society and its Future” (Kaczynski, 1995):

(§157) “Assuming that industrial society survives, it is likely that technology will eventually acquire something approaching complete control over human behavior. It has been established beyond any rational doubt that human thought and behavior have a largely biological basis. As experimenters have demonstrated, feelings such as hunger, pleasure, anger and fear can be turned on and off by electrical stimulation of appropriate parts of the brain. Memories can be destroyed by damaging parts of the brain or they can be brought to the surface by electrical stimulation. Hallucinations can be induced or moods changed by drugs. There may or may not be an immaterial human soul, but if there is one it clearly is less powerful that the biological mechanisms of human behavior. For if that were not the case then researchers would not be able so easily to manipulate human feelings and behavior with drugs and electrical currents.”
(§158) “It presumably would be impractical for all people to have electrodes inserted in their heads so that they could be controlled by the authorities. But the fact that human thoughts and feelings are so open to biological intervention shows that the problem of controlling human behavior is mainly a technical problem; a problem of neurons, hormones and complex molecules; the kind of problem that is accessible to scientific attack. Given the outstanding record of our society in solving technical problems, it is overwhelmingly probable that great advances will be made in the control of human behavior. Will public resistance prevent the introduction of technological control of human behavior? It certainly would if an attempt were made to introduce such control all at once. But since technological control will be introduced through a long sequence of small advances, there will be no rational and effective public resistance.”

This very brief historical discourse provides an informative background which emphasizes the enormous potential of scientific abuse of psychedelic substances, that is, for military dominance objectives, psychological manipulation, and for the purpose of cybernetic social engineering. Unfortunately, humanity has clearly not learned from history and immoral torture programs (which make perfidious use of psychology and neuroscience) are still being conducted in the 21st century, e.g., Abu Ghraib56F61F61F (Otterman, 2017). Only very recently APA psychologists were accused of psychological torture in the context of military operations which led to a public refusal of the APA to participate in future operations. In 2003, the CIA and the APA conducted a workshop (“Science of Deception: Integration of Practice and Theory”) which discussed the use of sensory overload and pharmacological interventions. According to Harper, workshop attendees were asked questions such as “What are the effects of sensory overload on the maintenance of deceptive behaviors? How might we overload the system or overwhelm the senses and see how it affects deceptive behaviors? What pharmacological agents are known to affect apparent truth-telling behavior?” In a post festum attempt to justify the leaked information the APA subsequently wrote a statement which later disappeared from their website : “The workshop provided an opportunity to bring together individuals with a need to understand and use deception in the service of national defense/security with those who investigate the phenomena and mechanisms of deception.” It has been argued that the “attempt to hide its history is not surprising, because the kind of activities discussed in these workshops are exactly like those that involved CIA and military mind control torture programs going back fifty years or more, and evidently still operational today” (Kaye, 2010).
5-Meo-DMT can be extremely destructive to the human psyche when it is utilized with the wrong intentions. It is therefore of utmost importance to develop legal frameworks which prevent its application in military settings and especially in “situations of crisis” and in matters of “national security”, so called “ticking time bomb scenarios” (Brecher, 2008) in which the principles laid down by the human rights convention (e.g., Geneva Conventions and the U.N. Convention Against Torture) are deemed to be no longer applicable for utilitarian reasons (as implicitly argued, for instance, by the David Horowitz – founder of the conservative think tank “David Horowitz Freedom Center”). Here is an example of a ticking-bomb Gedankenexperiment58F63F63F (a hypothetical moral dilemma):
Suppose that a person with knowledge of an imminent terrorist attack, that will kill many people, is in the hands of the authorities and that he will disclose the information needed to prevent the attack only if he is tortured. Should he be tortured? Should the most powerful psychoactive substances known to science (in casu, 5-MeO-DMT) be utilized to completely break down the persons sense of self for the purpose of “enhanced” interrogation?
In the psychological literature on morality similar provocative decision-making scenarios have been experimentally investigated in extenso, e.g., the “trolley problem” and the “foot-bridge dilemma” (more recently in virtual reality environments in order to increase external real-world validity and generalizability of results). There are generally two standpoints on morality: 1) a utilitarian view and 2) a deontological view. We maintain that torture should be denied on principle moral and ethical grounds. That is, we argue from a deontological stance (à la Immanuel Kant) as opposed to a utilitarian stance (à la David Hume). Specifically, we submit that there are certain moral and ethical boundaries which should never be transgressed, independent of the contextual circumstances (viz., moral absolutism vs. moral relativism). This is a meta-ethical position based on a priori principles of moral rationalism.
There are several rather complex “special case” objections to Kant’s categorical imperative but, as a general abstract heuristic, the underlying moral principle is very accurate. It also concords with Mahatma Gandhi’s philosophy of nonviolence (Sanskrit: Ahimsā)60F65F65F which is rooted in ancient Indian Vedanta philosophy and asserts that violence against another conscious being is never justifiable, under no circumstances (categorically). We regard nonviolence as a moral axiom (an aprioristic foundational moral primitive which defies further reduction) in line with Kant’s unconditional stance.

“Handle nur nach derjenigen Maxime, durch die du zugleich wollen kannst, dass sie ein allgemeines Gesetz werde.”
Transl.: “Act only according to that maxim whereby you can at the same time will that it should become a universal law.”
~ Immanuel Kant (1785), Grounding for the Metaphysics of Morals61F66F66F

The application of 5-MeO-DMT in a military context can have disastrous consequences because the compound completely breaks down psychological defence mechanisms. A person under the influence of 5-MeO-DMT is utterly defenceless and the “interrogator” has consequently “god-like sovereignty” (cf. Améry, 1966). Stimuli which are normally perceived as relatively harmless can be perceived as extremely threatening and their impact can be synergistically amplified in unpredictable ways – thereby causing irreversible psychological traumata. (We use the word trauma in the etymological sense — a psychological wound.) Physical torture is always also psychological torture, but it leaves open the theoretical possibility to distance (dissociate) psychologically from the torturer which allows in principle for a partial coping with the traumata. On the contrary, psychological torture targets the very core of a human being and therefore destroys the entire person and not “just” his/her physical body. In the hands of malignant individuals 5-MeO-DMT can be an extremely cruel and destructive neuropsychological weapon which can induce a form of permanent damage which is unimaginable to a normal person as it intervenes into the deepest core dynamics of consciousness.
Given the rapid breakdown of 5-MeO-DMT within the human organism (pharmacokinetic elimination) it is in principle difficult to prove its illegal application post festum. It follows on legal grounds that an absolute (universal) prohibition of the use of 5-MeO-DMT for military purposes is of great importance (no “margin of discretion”). Nullum crimen, nulla poena sine lege (Jimenez de Asúa, 1951; Rauter, 2017). This is especially pertinent in the present historical context in which basic human rights have been repeatedly violated behind the façade of dubious political motives (e.g., within the justificatory/exculpatory utilitarian frame of “national security”). We cannot allow for double standards and moral elasticity when it comes to human consciousness itself!67F67F

Conclusion
We would like to close by reconnecting the topic back to the introduction of this essay. In a recent PNAS article entitled “Trajectories of the Earth System in the Anthropocene” it has been stated that: “Collective human action is required to steer the Earth System […] Such action entails […] behavioral changes, technological innovations, new governance arrangements, and transformed social values” (Steffen et al., 2018, p. 8252).
We strongly agree with this general conclusion (note that the term “Earth System” is used in the singular not in the plural) and we argue that solidarity, collectivism, holism, morality, and altruism (as opposed to competition, individualism, atomism, corruption, and egocentrism) are pivotal for the evolution of humanity on this planet which Buckminster Fuller metaphorically labelled as “spaceship earth” (Anker, 2007) in an attempt to emphasize the common fate of all “passengers”. Fuller wrote the following in his final work entitled “Critical Path” (1981):

“History shows that, only when the leaders of the world’s great power structures have become convinced that their power structures are in danger of being destroyed, have the gargantuanly large, adequate funds been appropriated for accomplishing the necessary epoch-opening new technologies. It took preparation for World War III to make available the funds that have given us computers, transistors, rockets and satellites to realistically explore the Universe.”

A similar argument could be articulated for the “inner universe”. Humankind has travelled through outer space and we now possess detailed charts of the moon and many other extra-terrestrial objects. However, hitherto modern science is utterly unable to provide a comprehensive cartography of the human mind (let alone consciousness). Ergo, the great frontiers of 21st century science are internal and psychological, and it should be psychologies primary focus to systematically chart the largely unexplored “antipodes of mind” – the “terra incognita” – as Aldous Huxley eloquently formulated it (Huxley, 1954). In view of this it has been effectively argued that the discovery of psychedelic neuroactive substances is scientifically as important to the study of the mind as the invention of the microscope to progress in chemistry or the telescope to astronomy (Grof, 2000, p. 297). We would like to foreground that psychology is not reducible to neuroscience — it is a different level of analysis (the persistent “post hoc ergo propter hoc” fallacy is widespread; correlation ≠ causation).
Given the “extraordinary danger of the current moment”62 it is undeniable that we as human beings need to radically change our egoistic behaviour as a species, otherwise our existence on this planet will come to a catastrophic end soon. It is of pivotal momentousness to unveil perfidious attempts which try to exploit the present potential for positive change for the justification of the implementation of the neoliberal agenda which thrives for further global centralization and hence concentration of power (Coleman, 2005; Mausfeld, 2017; Smith & Chomsky, 1987). That is, real (or strategically self-created) problems are used to legitimate measures (under a false pretence) which would otherwise not be admissable (cf. the politician’s syllogism). History has taught humanity that prima facie seemingly well-intended motives have often been abused for the consolidation of power, e.g., the “war on terror” (Melley, 2017). The egocentric, competitive, and intrinsically antidemocratic principles of neoliberal capitalism have invaded all domains of life (Mausfeld, 2019). Domains in which they are utterly inappropriate and obviously destructive, such as the educational system,3F69F69F the health care system, geriatric care, the family systems, and even the scientific endeavour itself64F70F70F (cf. military Keynesianism within the military-academic complex65F71F71F ). The egocentric neoliberal doctrine has thus deeply influenced all aspects of human cognition and behaviour. As an implicit ideology it has been gradually assimilated and is therefore largely imperceptible (indoctrinated ideologies govern behaviour primarily via unconscious processes). Ludwig Wittgenstein termed this type of imperceptibility “aspect blindness”, e.g., a fish cannot see water. Sheldon Wolin (*1922;†2015) proposed the term “inverted totalitarianism” to describe a situation in which suppression and servitude are inversely perceived as the highest form of freedom because the mind is no longer capable to make any meaningful comparisons as it is no longer able to even imagine any alternatives to the dominant status quo (imagination and creativity are cognate, ergo, the systemic antagonism of these higher-order mental faculties consolidates the status quo).
Aldous Huxley foresaw this development of psychologically manufactured consensus in a lecture entitled “The Final Revolution” delivered at the University of Berkley in 1962:

“There will be, in the next generation or so, a pharmacological method of making people love their servitude, and producing dictatorship without tears, so to speak, producing a kind of painless concentration camp for entire societies, so that people will in fact have their liberties taken away from them, but will rather enjoy it, because they will be distracted from any desire to rebel by propaganda or brainwashing, or brainwashing enhanced by pharmacological methods. And this seems to be the final revolution.”

In the present tense, Zeese & Flowers (2014) characterized the situation as follows:

“We are living in a time of Inverted Totalitarianism, in which the tools used to maintain the status quo are much more subtle and technologically advanced. These include propaganda and control of the major media outlets that hide the real news about conditions at home and our activities around the world behind distractions such as high-profile citizen trials and celebrity gossip. The major electronic media, owned by six corporations nationwide, also routinely misinforms the public about domestic and foreign policy. A recent example is the “Fiscal Cliff.” Another tool is to create insecurity in the population so that people are unwilling to speak out and take risks for fear of losing their jobs and being unable to afford food, a home and health care. Changes in the work environment, such as the attack on unions and the war on whistleblowers, have led to greater job insecurity. Changes in college education also silence dissent, including the trend toward adjunct rather than tenured professors. Adjunct professors, now comprising 85 percent of faculty, are less willing to teach topics that are viewed as controversial. This, combined with massive student debt, are tools to silence the student population, once the center of transformative action.”

These quotations indicate that the problems humanity is facing are not confined to the physical sphere (i.e., militarisation and loss of biodiversity) but that the obliteration and eradication is furthermore psychological. The neoliberal agenda has devastating effects on human morals, values, and behaviours. It will take significant longitudinal collective efforts to counteract its cumulative effects on the human psyche, or else future generations will become incapacitated to even conceive alternative ways of thinking because the status quo is the only Weltanschauung they have ever known.
Behaviour is governed by thought and the basis of thought is consciousness. Ergo, the deduced essential question is: How can human consciousness be transformed for the better to change the trajectories of the Earth System and to enable a sustainable and free future for mankind? Science (and, ipso facto, particularly psychology and neuroscience) plays a central rôle in answering this question and a systematic investigation of the neurochemical substratum of consciousness (i.e., the essence of humanness) is consequently a decisive research agenda which should be prioritized. Technological progress and economic growth clearly cannot solve the present problems – they are a part of it (mechanistic/materialistic solutions cannot substitute for the lack of love which lies at the very heart of the problem at hand). So far, contemporary science has largely neglected the extraordinary experiences catalysed by various naturally-occurring psychedelic materials, some of which have been used as “neurocognitive tools” since time immemorial across numerous cultures for the purpose of healing and divination, in order to create states of communion, empathy, and transcendence, and to facilitate deep inter- and intrapersonal insights (Jones, 2007; Tupper, 2002). Further, the potential of the extremely powerful endogenous neurochemical 5-MeO-DMT has not yet been explored at all (it is wide-open uncharted scientific territory). Specifically, research in the domain of creativity appears to be vital for species survival because humanity needs to find alternative ways of existence. If there is a chance that endogenous neurochemicals such as 5-MeO-DMT can catalyse a radical novel (less egocentric and more loving) creative way of thinking which fosters biophilia, egalitarian attitudes, solidarity, empathy, altruism, emotional intelligence, and noetic epistemological insights into the interconnectedness of nature, and indeed all of existence, then it is sciences moral obligation to take this potential very seriously as creative change is de facto an evolutionary matter of paramountcy. The transformational ego-dissolving experience of nonduality might prove to be the quintessential antidote to the rigid, habitual, materialistic, dualistic, and egoic mindset which lies at the very core of the existential crisis.66F72F72F That is, an egoic and competitive mindset is incompatible with the imperative need for collective action and the interpersonal effects of transpersonal experiences might provide significant impetus to realize this communal goal. The transformation of deep-seated plutonic self-identity schemata (i.e., how human creatures conceive of themselves) is perhaps the most pivotal aspect. The culturally conditioned dualistic psychological schism which separates man from nature (Fromm, 1962) stands in sharp contrast with an integral, interconnected, unitive, and holistic worldview.
Recently a bipartite model of 5-HT signalling has been proposed which is pertinent in this regard (Carhart-Harris & Nutt, 2017). It has been suggested that passive coping67F73F73F (stress moderation via passive toleration of the stressor) is the brains default modus (status quo) which is mediated via 5-HT1A receptor signalling. Active coping (engaging and changing the cause of stress), on the other hand, is conjectured to be mediated via 5-HT2A signalling. Specifically, the authors proposed that 5-HT2A-mediated plasticity may be crucial “as the level of adversity reaches a critical point”. The concept “plasticity” was generically defined as the “capacity for change” (but see Carhart-Harris & Nutt, 2017).
With respect to paradigm-change (i.e., a shift from a dualistic egocentric tier to an interconnected unitive tier) it is important to note that in the past “revolutions of thought” have been systematically subverted by the neoliberal “financial power élite” (Harvey, 2007; Hill & Kumar, 2009; Mausfeld, 2019) which invests heavily into studies of social psychology, group dynamics, and analyses of social movements (Mausfeld, 2017; Sugarman, 2015). Paradigm-shifting social energy is intentionally diffused and systematically redirected towards substitutional “strawman objectives”. Movements are infiltrated, “Red Herring strategies” are employed, and effective activists are coopted or otherwise socially discredited via various ad hominem arguments as a persona non grata. By this psychological stratagem real change, which targets the roots of the problem, is prevented. In brevi, change is intentionally inhibited by those who benefit from the corrupt status quo and powerful slogans such as “save the planet” and “change for a better future” are taken advantage of to expedite the ongoing process of centralisation of power. Tactics to stymie effective change include cognitive infiltration (Sunstein & Vermeule, 2008, 2009), internal/external coöptation, exploitation of the principles of group-dynamics such as group-pressure and conformity, cybernetic methods such as algorithmic censorship of digital information, various forms of psychological manipulation such as induction of fear and distrust, etc. pp. (but see Chomsky, 2016). The antagonism between creativity and control is a long-standing issue which was central in the historical context of the 18th century enlightenment revolution which shares many characteristics with the current situation (Fromm, 1962). Humanistic motives and neoliberal capitalism are incompatible. The former seeks freedom and equality while the latter is based on control and exploitation. Creativity is fundamentally based on freedom and liberty. Per contrast, neoliberal control is based on power and suppression. Both are diametrically opposed cannot coexist in harmony (Gormley, 2018; Harvey, 2007).
Besides challenging the destructive egocentric status quo, epistemological insights into the nondual ontology of existence (e.g., dual-aspect monism/neutral monism)68F74F74F challenge some of the most central assumptions of contemporary mainstream science, e.g., the notion of detached scientific objectivity which is a cognitive/epistemic illusion (Hoffman, 2016; cf. Wiseman, 2015). A nondual conceptualization of reality might force us to rethink our axiomatic (non-evidence based and naïve) doxa (Bourdieu, 1977) about the way we conceive reality and practice science, e.g., the stipulated dichotomy between observer and observed69F75F75F and the widespread belief that the brain produces consciousness.70F76F76F A nondual reconceptualization is therefore implicitly perceived as a threat to the widely adopted “quasi-Newtonian” status quo which has in reality already been fundamentally revised by modern quantum physics, viz., the widely held and mainly unquestioned metaphysical assumption of local realism is no longer empirically tenable (Handsteiner et al., 2017; Schlosshauer, Kofler, & Zeilinger, 2013). A large proportion of mainstream science still operates under an outdated deterministic Newtonian paradigm (from a Bayesian epistemology perspective an update of priors in the light of new empirical evidence is required). “Belief bias” plays a central part in this irrational situation (Evans et al., 1993). In his seminal book “The structure of scientific revolutions” Thomas Kuhn pointed out that it is a general phenomenon that paradigm challenging anomalies “that subvert the existing tradition of scientific practice” (T. Kuhn, 1970, p. 6) are neglected as long as possible. Along the same line Abraham Maslow discussed the “Psychology of Science” in great detail in his eponymous book (Maslow, 1962). Maslow formulated a quasi-Gödelian critique of orthodox science and its “unproved articles of faith, and taken-for-granted definitions, axioms, and concepts”. Fundamental research on extremely powerful consciousness-expanding substances like 5-MeO-DMT might thus force us the rethink largely unquestioned axiomatic epistemological and ontological assumptions. Prima vista, this critical line of thought might sound far-fetched and even absurd but such an argumentum ad lapidem constitutes no valid reason for the prima facie rejection of the idea.
From an anthropological perspective, it might be helpful to look at the way indigenous cultures utilised 5-MeO-DMT traditionally and how they related to the Earth System, to each other, and to the Self (i.e., psyche and physis). From an evolutionary vantage point it might be argued that ego-dissolving psychoactive plants and fungi played an important rôle in the comparably harmonious (symbiotic) relationship which governed man and nature before the industrial revolution (McKenna, 1992). Again, such a proposal predictably appears absurd to the modern (hyper-technological) mind. However, the present situation is more than merely absurd — it is clearly pathological (e.g., nuclear weapons positioned all over the globe – ready to launch at any time). Any real solution to the “anthropogenic global crisis” will be at odds with the predominant status quo and will thus cause intensely virulent cognitive dissonance in the minds of most passively-obedient dogmatic status quo followers (cf. Sir Francis Bacon’s analogy on “ants, spiders, and bees” expounded in his Novum Organum, 1620).
From a general philosophy of science and set-theoretical perspective it can be cogently argued that if science wants to live up to its ideal to capture reality in its entirety, without leaving any residue, it needs to integrate neurochemicals like 5-MeO-DMT into its modelling efforts – especially given the fact that this alkaloid is an endogenous components of the human brain and, ergo, in all likelihood of evolutionary relevance (the rôle of 5-MeO-DMT in neurobiology is currently utterly elusive and we argue that neurochemical vestigiality is an unlikely explanation). In conclusio, any model which incorporates only a specific (a priori selected) subset of the available quantitative and qualitative data is ipso facto at best incomplete (and in the worst-case scenario prejudiced, dogmatic, and systematically biased). We are confident that a mature science will sooner or later investigate 5-MeO-DMT in the context of human psychology and neurobiology. It is just a matter of time — and of neuropolitics (cf. Rose & Abi-Rached, 2014). Nil desperandum. Tempora mutantur, nos et mutamur in illis – or to use the better-known Greek semantic equivalent: Panta rhei. The fact that we are living in a world which can change let Leibniz to the conclusion that we are living in le meilleur des mondes possible.
We will end with three thematically interrelated quotations from the distinguished polymath William James who was intrinsically interested in mystical and transcendental experiences (as evidenced, inter alia, by his excellent book “The varieties of religious experience”). In the introduction of his essay “The hidden Self” James articulated the following (a quotation which might surprise those who stereotype James as a prototypical exemplar of American pragmatism, while, in actuality, his versatility and open-mindedness defies social categorization):

“The scientific-academic mind and the feminine-mystical mind shy from each other’s facts, just as they shy from each other’s temper and spirit. Facts are there only for those who have a mental affinity with them. When once they are indisputably ascertained and admitted, the academic and critical minds are by far the best fitted ones to interpret and discuss them – for surely to pass from mystical to scientific speculations is like passing from lunacy to sanity; but on the other hand if there is anything which human history demonstrates, it is the extreme slowness with which the ordinary academic and critical mind acknowledges facts to exist which present themselves as wild facts with no stall or pigeon-hole, or as facts which threaten to break up the accepted system. In psychology, physiology, and medicine, wherever a debate between the Mystics and the Scientifics has been once for all decided, it is the Mystics who have usually proved to be right about the facts, while the Scientifics had the better of it in respect to the theories.” (James, 1890, pp. 361–362)

James conducted self-experiments with the gaseous chemical compound nitrous oxide and the mescaline72F78F78F containing psychedelic cactus “peyote” (Lophophora williamsii). He was enthusiastic about the effects of nitrous oxide (which is not a genuine psychedelic) and it has been argued that his first-hand experiences with this dissociative anaesthetic played a central rôle for the development of some of his most central ideas on mysticism and revelation (Moon, Kuza, & Desai, 2018). However, his experimentation with the truly visionary peyote was unfortunately unsuccessful. One can only speculate: Which turn would Western psychology have taken if James’ mind would have entered the psychedelic realm? James concluded his paper on nitrous oxide inhalation as follows:

“It seems, indeed, a causa sui, or ‘spirit become its own object’. My conclusion is that the togetherness of things in a common world, the law of sharing, of which I have said so much, may, when perceived, engender a very powerful emotion; that Hegel was so unusually succeptible [sic] to this emotion; throughout his life that its gratification became his supreme end, and made him tolerably unscrupulous as to the means he employed; that indifferentism is the true outcome of every view of the world which makes infinity and continuity to be its essence, and that pessimistic or optimistic attitudes pertain to the more accidental subjectivity of the moment; finally, that the identification of contradictories, so far from being the self-developing process which Hegel supposes, is really a self-consuming process, passed from the less to the more abstract, and terminating either in a laugh at the ultimate nothingness, or in a mood of vertiginous amazement at a meaningless infinity.” (James, 1882)

Years later, James eloquently articulated the importance of unbiased and eclectic inquiry in light of his “radical empiricism” stance. The entire disquisition is presented in his essay “A World of Pure Experience”:

“To be radical, an empiricism must neither admit into its constructions any element that is not directly experienced, nor exclude from them any element that is directly experienced. For such a philosophy, the relations that connect experiences must themselves be experienced relations, and any kind of relation experienced must be accounted as ‘real’ as anything else in the system. Elements may indeed be redistributed, the original placing of things getting corrected, but a real place must be found for every kind of thing experienced, whether term or relation, in the final philosophic arrangement.” (James, 1904, p. 534)

Conflict of interest statement

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References
Agurell, S., Holmstedt, B., Lindgren, J.-E., Schultes, R. E., Lindberg, A. A., Jansen, G., … Samuelsson, B. (1969). Alkaloids in Certain Species of Virola and Other South American Plants of Ethnopharmacologic Interest. Acta Chemica Scandinavica, 23, 903–916.
Akers, B. P., Ruiz, J. F., Piper, A., & Ruck, C. A. P. (2011). A Prehistoric Mural in Spain Depicting Neurotropic Psilocybe Mushrooms? Economic Botany, 65(2), 121–128.
al-Rifaie, M. M., Bishop, J. M., & Caines, S. (2012). Creativity and Autonomy in Swarm Intelligence Systems. Cognitive Computation, 4(3), 320–331.
Alda, M., Puebla-Guedea, M., Rodero, B., Demarzo, M., Montero-Marin, J., Roca, M., & Garcia-Campayo, J. (2016). Zen meditation, Length of Telomeres, and the Role of Experiential Avoidance and Compassion. Mindfulness, 7(3), 651–659.
Altvater, E. (2016). The Capitalocene, or, Geoengineering against Capitalism’s Planetary Boundaries. In Anthropocene or Capitalocene? Nature, History, and the Crisis of Capitalism. Oakland: PM Press.
Amin, A. H., Crawford, T. B. B., & Gaddum, J. H. (1954). The distribution of substance P and 5-hydroxytryptamine in the central nervous system of the dog. Journal of Physiology, 596–618.
Anderson, T., Petranker, R., Rosenbaum, D., Weissman, C. R., Dinh-Williams, L. A., Hui, K., … Farb, N. A. S. (2019). Microdosing psychedelics: personality, mental health, and creativity differences in microdosers. Psychopharmacology, 236(2), 731–740.
Anjana, M. P. (2019). NEURALINK Implantation of Artificial Intelligence in Humanbeings. International Journal of Computer Sciences and Engineering.
Anker, P. (2007). Buckminster Fuller as Captain of Spaceship Earth. Minerva, 45(4), 417–434.
Anokhin, A. P., Golosheykin, S., Grant, J. D., & Heath, A. C. (2011). Heritability of Delay Discounting in Adolescence: A Longitudinal Twin Study. Behavior Genetics, 41(2), 175–183.
Armbrust, C., & Chomsky, N. (2005). Hegemony or Survival: America’s Quest for Global Dominance. The Antioch Review, 63(3), 594.
Arndt, J., & Vess, M. (2008). Tales from Existential Oceans: Terror Management Theory and How the Awareness of Our Mortality Affects Us All. Social and Personality Psychology Compass.
Arquilla, J. (2011). From blitzkrieg to bitskrieg. Communications of the ACM, 54(10), 58.
Atmanspacher, H. (1997). Cartesian cut, Heisenberg cut, and the concept of complexity. World Futures, 49(3), 333–355.
Atmanspacher, H. (2012). Dual-aspect monism à la Pauli and Jung perforates the completeness of physics. In Journal of Consciousness Studies (pp. 5–21).
Aziz, V. M., & Warner, N. J. (2005). Capgras’ syndrome of time. Psychopathology.
Azmitia, E. C. (2012). Global Projections of Ancient Aromatics Neurotransmitters. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Human Behavior. Elsevier Science & Technology Books.
Baas, M., Nevicka, B., & Ten Velden, F. S. (2014). Specific Mindfulness Skills Differentially Predict Creative Performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by Distraction. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1117–1122.
Baird, J. C. (1997). Sensation and judgement: complementarity theory of psychophysics. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bamford, J. (2001). Body of secrets: anatomy of the ultra-secret National Security Agency : from the Cold War through the dawn of a new century. Doubleday.
Barker, S. A., Borjigin, J., Lomnicka, I., & Strassman, R. (2013). LC/MS/MS analysis of the endogenous dimethyltryptamine hallucinogens, their precursors, and major metabolites in rat pineal gland microdialysate. Biomedical Chromatography, 27(12), 1690–1700.
Barnosky, A. D., Matzke, N., Tomiya, S., Wogan, G. O. U., Swartz, B., Quental, T. B., … Ferrer, E. A. (2011). Has the Earth’s sixth mass extinction already arrived? Nature.
Barrett, F. S., Bradstreet, M. P., Leoutsakos, J.-M. S., Johnson, M. W., & Griffiths, R. R. (2016). The Challenging Experience Questionnaire: Characterization of challenging experiences with psilocybin mushrooms. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 1279–1295.
Barrett, F. S., Johnson, M. W., & Griffiths, R. R. (2015). Validation of the revised Mystical Experience Questionnaire in experimental sessions with psilocybin. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(11), 1182–1190.
Barsuglia, J. P., Polanco, M., Palmer, R., Malcolm, B. J., Kelmendi, B., & Calvey, T. (2018). A case report SPECT study and theoretical rationale for the sequential administration of ibogaine and 5-MeO-DMT in the treatment of alcohol use disorder. In Progress in Brain Research (pp. 121–158).
Baudrillard, J. (1998). The consumer society: myths and structures. Sage Publications Ltd.
Baudrillard, J., & Lotringer, S. (2005). The conspiracy of art : manifestos, interviews, essays. Semiotext(e).
Bauman, Z., Bigo, D., Esteves, P., Guild, E., Jabri, V., Lyon, D., & Walker, R. B. J. (2014). After Snowden: Rethinking the impact of surveillance. International Political Sociology.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The Need to Belong: Desire for Interpersonal Attachments as a Fundamental Human Motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351–355.
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative Cognition and Brain Network Dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 87–95.
Beaty, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Benedek, M., Chen, Q., … Silvia, P. J. (2018). Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(5), 1087–1092.
Benedek, M., Könen, T., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Associative abilities underlying creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(3), 273–281.
Benoit, J., Abdala, F., Manger, P., & Rubidge, B. (2016). The sixth sense in mammalians forerunners: variability of the parietal foramen and the evolution of the pineal eye in South African Permo-Triassic eutheriodont therapsids. Acta Palaeontologica Polonica.
Bentley, P. J., & Corne, D. W. (2002). An introduction to Creative Evolutionary Systems. In Creative Evolutionary Systems (pp. 1–75). Elsevier.
Bernays, E. L. (1928). Propaganda. Horace Liveright.
Berra, T. M., Alvarez, G., & Shannon, K. (2010). The Galton-Darwin-Wedgwood Pedigree of H. H. Laughlin. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 101(1), 228–241.
Beucke, J. C., Sepulcre, J., Eldaief, M. C., Sebold, M., Kathmann, N., & Kaufmann, C. (2014). Default mode network subsystem alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry.
Biehl-Missal, B., & Saren, M. (2012). Atmospheres of seduction: A critique of aesthetic marketing practices. Journal of Macromarketing.
Bizzotto, J. (2018). The hypothesis on the presence of entheogens in the Eleusinian Mysteries. Medicina Historica, 2(2), 85–93.
Blechner, M. J. (2001). The dream frontier. Analytic Press.
Block, N., & Stalnaker, R. (1999). Conceptual Analysis, Dualism, and the Explanatory Gap. The Philosophical Review, 108(1), 1.
Bob, P., & Louchakova, O. (2015). Dissociative states in dreams and brain chaos: implications for creative awareness. Frontiers in Psychology, 6.
Boden, M. A. (2014). Creativity and Artificial Intelligence. In The Philosophy of Creativity (pp. 224–244). Oxford University Press.
Bogenschutz, M. P., & Forcehimes, A. A. (2017). Development of a Psychotherapeutic Model for Psilocybin-Assisted Treatment of Alcoholism. Journal of Humanistic Psychology.
Bogenschutz, M. P., Forcehimes, A. A., Pommy, J. A., Wilcox, C. E., Barbosa, P. C. R., & Strassman, R. J. (2015). Psilocybin-assisted treatment for alcohol dependence : A proof-of-concept study. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(3), 289–299.
Bogenschutz, M. P., & Johnson, M. W. (2016). Classic hallucinogens in the treatment of addictions. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 64, 250–258.
Bohannon, J. (2015). Fears of an AI pioneer. Science, 349(6245), 252–252.
Boire, R. (2000). On Cognitive Liberty. Journal of Cognitive Liberties, 1(1), 1–26.
Bolstridge, M. (2013). The Psychedelic Renaissance: Reassessing the Role of Psychedelic Drugs in 21st Century Psychiatry and Society. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(3), 239–239.
Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge University Press.
Brecher, B. (2008). Torture and the Ticking Bomb. Torture and the Ticking Bomb.
Breggin, P. R. (1998). Electroshock: scientific, ethical, and political issues. International Journal of Risk & Safety in Medicine.
Breggin, P. R. (2003). Psychopharmacology And Human Values. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 43(2), 34–49.
Breggin, P. R. (2008). Brain-disabling treatments in psychiatry: Drugs, electroshock, and the psychopharmaceutical complex (2nd ed.). Brain-Disabling Treatments in Psychiatry: Drugs, Electroshock, and the Psychopharmaceutical Complex (2nd Ed.). Springer Pub.
Breggin, P. R. (2011). Psychiatric drug-induced Chronic Brain Impairment (CBI): Implications for long-term treatment with psychiatric medication. International Journal of Risk and Safety in Medicine, 23(4), 193–200.
Breggin, P. R. (2016). Rational Principles of Psychopharmacology for Therapists, Healthcare Providers and Clients. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 46(1), 1–13.
Breggin, P. R., & Breggin, G. R. (1996). The Hazards of Treating “Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder” with Methylphenidate (Ritalin). Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 10(2), 55–72.
Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y.-Y., Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
Brown, R. E. (2007). Alfred McCoy, Hebb, the CIA and Torture. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 43(2), 205–213.
Brzezinski, Z. (1997). The grand chessboard: American primacy and its geostrategic imperatives. BasicBooks.
Burch, G. S. J., Hemsley, D. R., Pavelis, C., & Corr, P. J. (2006). Personality, creativity and latent inhibition. European Journal of Personality, 20(2), 107–122.
Burgarth, D., Facchi, P., Giovannetti, V., Nakazato, H., Pascazio, S., & Yuasa, K. (2014). Quantum Computing in Plato’s Cave.
Burnyeat, M. (2000). Plato on Why Mathematics is Good for the Soul. Proceedings of the British Academy, 103, 1–83.
Byron, K., Khazanchi, S., & Nazarian, D. (2010). The relationship between stressors and creativity: A meta-analysis examining competing theoretical models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 201–212.
Callaway, J. C., Grob, C. S., McKenna, D. J., Nichols, D. E., Shulgins, A., & Tupper, K. W. (2006). A Demand for Clarity Regarding a Case Report on the Ingestion of 5-Methoxy-N, N-Dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT) in an Ayahuasca Preparation. Journal of Analytical Toxicology, 30(6), 406–407.
Cameron, L. P., & Olson, D. E. (2018). Dark Classics in Chemical Neuroscience: N , N -Dimethyltryptamine (DMT). ACS Chemical Neuroscience.
Capurso, V., Fabbro, F., & Crescentini, C. (2014). Mindful creativity: the influence of mindfulness meditation on creative thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.
Carbonaro, T. M., & Gatch, M. B. (2016). Neuropharmacology of N,N-dimethyltryptamine. Brain Research Bulletin, 126(1), 74–88.
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Bolstridge, M., Rucker, J., Day, C. M. J., Erritzoe, D., Kaelen, M., … Nutt, D. J. (2016). Psilocybin with pyschological support for treatment-resistant depression: an open-label feasibility study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(7), 619–627.
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Erritzoe, D., Haijen, E., Kaelen, M., & Watts, R. (2018). Psychedelics and connectedness. Psychopharmacology, 235(2), 547–550.
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Erritzoe, D., Williams, T., Stone, J. M., Reed, L. J., Colasanti, A., … Nutt, D. J. (2012). Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(6), 2138–2143.
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Leech, R., Hellyer, P. J., Shanahan, M., Feilding, A., Tagliazucchi, E., … Nutt, D. (2014). The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(2), 20.
Carhart-Harris, R. L., & Nutt, D. (2017). Serotonin and brain function: A tale of two receptors. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 31(9), 1091–1120.
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Roseman, L., Bolstridge, M., Demetriou, L., Pannekoek, J. N., Wall, M. B., … Nutt, D. J. (2017). Psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression: FMRI-measured brain mechanisms. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 13187.
Carlozzi, A. F., Bull, K. S., Eells, G. T., & Hurlburt, J. D. (1995). Empathy as related to creativity, dogmatism, and expressiveness. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied.
Cartwright, J. H. E., & González, D. L. (2016). Möbius Strips Before Möbius: Topological Hints in Ancient Representations. Mathematical Intelligencer.
Casey, G., & Moran, A. (1989). The Computational Metaphor and Cognitive Psychology. The Irish Journal of Psychology.
Casselman, I., Nock, C. J., Wohlmuth, H., Weatherby, R. P., & Heinrich, M. (2014). From local to global—Fifty years of research on Salvia divinorum. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 151(2), 768–783.
Catlow, B. J., Song, S., Paredes, D. A., Kirstein, C. L., & Sanchez-Ramos, J. (2013). Effects of psilocybin on hippocampal neurogenesis and extinction of trace fear conditioning. Experimental Brain Research, 228(4), 481–491.
Cavanna, A. E. (2007). The Precuneus and Consciousness. CNS Spectrums, 12(7), 545–552.
Ceballos, G., & Ehrlich, P. R. (2018). The misunderstood sixth mass extinction. Science.
Ceballos, G., Ehrlich, P. R., & Dirzo, R. (2017). Biological annihilation via the ongoing sixth mass extinction signaled by vertebrate population losses and declines. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
Chakravartty, P., & Schiller, D. a N. (2010). Neoliberal Newspeak and Digital Capitalism in Crisis. International Journal of Communication, 4, 670–692.
Chalmers, D. J. (1995). Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness. Journal of Conscious Studies, 2(3), 200–219.
Chase, A. (2000). Harvard and the Making of the Unabomber. The Atlantic, 285(6), 41–63.
Chen, A. L., & Chen, K. K. (1939). Harmine, the alkaloid of caapi. Quarterly Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology.
Chen, T.-S., Liou, S.-Y., & Chang, Y.-L. (2008). Antioxidant Evaluation of Three Adaptogen Extracts. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine.
Chen, Y., Ozturk, N. C., & Zhou, F. C. (2013). DNA Methylation Program in Developing Hippocampus and Its Alteration by Alcohol. PLoS ONE, 8(3).
Chomsky, N. (1992). Manufacturing Consent: Noam Chomsky and the Media. East.
Chomsky, N. (2015). Academic Freedom and the Subservience to Power. In Who’s Afraid of Academic Freedom?
Chomsky, N. (2016). Who rules the world? Hamish Hamilton .
Claridge, G., & Blakey, S. (2009). Schizotypy and affective temperament: Relationships with divergent thinking and creativity styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(8), 820–826.
Coleman, J. (2005). The Corporation – The pathological Pursuit of Power and Profit. European Business Review, 17(5), ebr.2005.05417eab.005.
Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to Create: The Impact of Focused-Attention and Open-Monitoring Training on Convergent and Divergent Thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3.
Comai, S., Bertazzo, A., Carretti, N., Podfigurna-Stopa, A., Luisi, S., & Costa, C. V. L. (2010). Serum Levels of Tryptophan, 5-Hydroxytryptophan and Serotonin in Patients Affected with Different Forms of Amenorrhea. International Journal of Tryptophan Research, 3, IJTR.S3804.
Comfort, A. (1979). The Cartesian observer revisited: ontological implications of the homuncular illusion. Journal of Social and Biological Systems, 2(3), 211–223.
Cooper, N. (1966). The Importance of Δianoia in Plato’s Theory of Forms. The Classical Quarterly, 16(1), 65–69.
Cozzi, N. V. (2013). Psychedelic Breakthroughs in Neuroscience : How Psychedelic Drugs Influenced the Growth and Development of Psychopharmacology. MAPS Special Bulletin.
Crick, F. (1970). Central dogma of molecular biology. Nature.
Cropley, D. H., Cropley, A. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Runco, M. A. (2010). The Dark Side of Creativity. (D. H. Cropley, A. J. Cropley, J. C. Kaufman, & M. A. Runco, Eds.), The Dark Side of Creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
d’Inverno, M., & Luck, M. (2012). Creativity Through Autonomy and Interaction. Cognitive Computation, 4(3), 332–346.
Da Lee, R., Mi An, S., Sun Kim, S., Seek Rhee, G., Jun Kwack, S., Hyun Seok, J., … Lea Park, K. (2005). Neurotoxic Effects of Alcohol and Acetaldehyde During Embryonic Development. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part A, 68(23–24), 2147–2162.
Dakic, V. (2017). Short term changes in the proteome of human cerebral organoids induced by 5-methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine. BIoRxlv.
Dakic, V., Minardi Nascimento, J., Costa Sartore, R., Maciel, R. de M., de Araujo, D. B., Ribeiro, S., … Rehen, S. K. (2017). Short term changes in the proteome of human cerebral organoids induced by 5-MeO-DMT. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 12863.
Davis, J. V., & Canty, J. M. (2013). Ecopsychology and Transpersonal Psychology. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Transpersonal Psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 595–611). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Davis, W., & Weil, A. T. (1992). Identity of a New World Psychoactive Toad. Ancient Mesoamerica, 3(1), 51–59.
Dawson, J. (2004). Reflectivity, Creativity, and the Space for Silence. Reflective Practice.
DeFries, R., & Nagendra, H. (2017). Ecosystem management as a wicked problem. Science, 356(6335), 265–270.
Delgado, J. M. R. (1964). Free Behavior and Brain Stimulation. In International Review of Neurobiology (pp. 349–449).
Delgado, J. M. R., & Hamlin, H. (1956). Surface and depth electrography of the frontal lobes in conscious patients. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 8(3), 371–384.
DeScioli, P., Christner, J., & Kurzban, R. (2011). The Omission Strategy. Psychological Science, 22(4), 442–446.
Deussen, P. (1906). The philosophy of the Upanishads. Cosimo Classics.
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of Openness/Intellect: Cognitive and Neuropsychological Correlates of the Fifth Factor of Personality. Journal of Personality, 73(4), 825–858.
Dias, B. G., & Ressler, K. J. (2014). Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nature Neuroscience, 17(1), 89–96.
Dietz, T., Rosa, E. A., & York, R. (2007). Driving the human ecological footprint. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment.
Ding, X., Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2014). Improving creativity performance by short-term meditation. Behavioral and Brain Functions.
Domhoff, G. (1975). New Directions in Power Structure Research (Worn edition). The Insurgent Sociologist.
Domino, G. (1976). Transcendental meditation and creativity: An empirical investigation. Journal of Applied Psychology.
Douglas, A., & Pond, C. (2012). America’s War on Drugs and the Prison-Industrial Complex. Gender Race & Justice, 417.
Eakin, R. M., & Westfall, J. A. (1959). Fine Structure of the Retina in the Reptilian Third Eye. The Journal of Cell Biology.
Eddington, A. S. (1929). The nature of the physical world. Book.
Eddington, A. S. (1938). The philosophy of physical science. Cambridge University Press.
El-Seedi, H. R., De Smet, P. A. G. M., Beck, O., Possnert, G., & Bruhn, J. G. (2005). Prehistoric peyote use: Alkaloid analysis and radiocarbon dating of archaeological specimens of Lophophora from Texas. Journal of Ethnopharmacology.
Elliston, J. (1999). Psywar on Cuba: the declassified history of U.S. anti-Castro propaganda. Ocean Press.
Ellul, J. (1973). Propaganda : the formation of men’s attitudes. Vintage Books.
Epel, E., Daubenmier, J., Moskowitz, J. T., Folkman, S., & Blackburn, E. (2009). Can meditation slow rate of cellular aging? Cognitive stress, mindfulness, and telomeres. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1172(1), 34–53.
Erspamer, V., Vitali, T., Roseghini, M., & Cei, J. M. (1965). 5-Methoxy- and 5-hydroxy-indolealkylamines in the skin of Bufo alvarius. Experientia, 21(9), 504.
Evans, J. S. B. T., Over, D. E., & Manktelow, K. I. (1993). Reasoning, decision making and rationality. Cognition.
Farah, M. J. (2005). Neuroethics: The practical and the philosophical. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
Faust, M., & Kenett, Y. N. (2014). Rigidity, chaos and integration: hemispheric interaction and individual differences in metaphor comprehension. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8.
Feist, G. J. (1998). A Meta-Analysis of Personality in Scientific and Artistic Creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290–309.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Scientific American.
Fleming, P., & Oswick, C. (2014). Educating consent? A conversation with Noam Chomsky on the university and business school education. Organization, 21(4), 568–578.
Fleming, S. M., Thomas, C. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Overcoming status quo bias in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(13), 6005–6009.
Forgeard, M. J. C. (2013). Perceiving benefits after adversity: The relationship between self-reported posttraumatic growth and creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 245–264.
Forstmann, M., & Sagioglou, C. (2017). Lifetime experience with (classic) psychedelics predicts pro-environmental behavior through an increase in nature relatedness. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 31(8), 975–988.
Foster, J. B., & Holleman, H. (2014). The Financial Power Elite. Monthly Review.
Francesc Alonso, J., Romero, S., Angel Mañanas, M., & Riba, J. (2015). Serotonergic psychedelics temporarily modify information transfer in humans. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 18(8), 1–9.
Frecska, E., Bokor, P., & Winkelman, M. (2016). The Therapeutic Potentials of Ayahuasca: Possible Effects against Various Diseases of Civilization. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 7.
Frederking, W. (1955). Intoxicant Drugs (Mescaline and Lysergic Acid Diethylamide) in psychotherapy. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 121(3), 262–266.
Freedman, J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (2017). Social Psychology in Natural Settings. Social Psychology in Natural Settings: A Reader in Field Experimentation. Routledge.
Freud, S. (1923). The Ego and the Id. The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Volume XIX (1923-1925): The Ego and the Id and Other Works, 19–27.
Freud, S. (1930). Civilisation and Its Discontents. In The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud – The future of an illusion, civilization and its discontents, and other works.
Freud, S. (1939). Die Traumdeutung. Leipzig und Wien: Franz Deuticke.
Fromm, E. (1962). Beyond the chains of illusion : my encounter with Marx and Freud. Continuum.
Fromm, E. (1976). To Have Or To Be? Bloomsbury.
Froud, J., Johal, S., Moran, M., & Williams, K. (2017). Outsourcing the State: New Sources of Elite Power. Theory, Culture & Society, 34(5–6), 77–101.
Fuller, R. B. (1981). Critical path. St. Martin’s Press.
Furst, P. T. (1972). Symbolism and Psychopharmacology: the Toad as Earth Mother in Indian America. In K.J. Litrak & T.N. Castillo (Eds.), 1972, Religión en Mesoamerica, XII Mesa Redonda, Sociedad Méxicana de Antropología, México (pp. 37–46).
Gable, R. S. (1993). Toward a comparative overview of dependence potential and acute toxicity of psychoactive substances used nonmedically. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 19(3), 263–281.
Gaddum, J. H., & Hameed, K. A. (1954). Drugs which antagonize 5-hydroxytryptamine. British Journal of Pharmacology, 240–248.
Geerling, W., Magee, G. B., & Brooks, R. (2015). Cooperation, defection and resistance in Nazi Germany. Explorations in Economic History, 58, 125–139.
Gentry, C. (1991). J. Edgar Hoover : the man and the secrets. Norton.
Gillis, J. (1966). Auguste Kekulé et son oeuvre, réalisée à Gand de 1858 à 1867. Bruxelles: Palais des académies.
Gilmore, R. W. (1999). Globalisation and US prison growth: from military Keynesianism to post-Keynesian militarism. Race & Class, 40(2–3), 171–188.
Gino, F., & Ariely, D. (2012). The dark side of creativity: Original thinkers can be more dishonest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Gino, F., & Wiltermuth, S. S. (2014). Evil Genius? How Dishonesty Can Lead to Greater Creativity. Psychological Science.
Giroux, H. A. (2015). University in Chains. University in Chains: Confronting the Military-Industrial-Academic Complex. Routledge.
Glennon, R. A., & Rosecrans, J. A. (1982). Indolealkylamine and phenalkylamine hallucinogens: A brief overview. In Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.
Glick, S. D., & Maisonneuve, I. M. (1998). Mechanisms of antiaddictive actions of ibogaine. In Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 844, pp. 214–226).
Glisky, M. L., Tataryn, D. J., Tobias, B. A., Kihlstrom, J. F., & McConkey, K. M. (1991). Absorption, openness to experience, and hypnotizability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 263–272.
Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of three-term relational reasoning. Neuropsychologia.
Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Gormley, K. (2018). Neoliberalism and the discursive construction of ‘creativity.’ Critical Studies in Education, 1–16.
Grady, B., & Loewenthal, K. M. (1997). Features associated with speaking in tongues (glossolalia). British Journal of Medical Psychology.
Grant, R. M. (Robert M. (1996). Irenaeus of Lyons, 169.
Gregg, A. P., Mahadevan, N., & Sedikides, C. (2017). Intellectual arrogance and intellectual humility: correlational evidence for an evolutionary-embodied-epistemological account. Journal of Positive Psychology, 12(1), 59–73.
Grey, A. (2001). Transfigurations. Rochester, VT: Inner Traditions.
Griffiths, R. R., Johnson, M. W., Carducci, M. A., Umbricht, A., Richards, W. A., Richards, B. D., … Klinedinst, M. A. (2016). Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized double-blind trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 1181–1197.
Griffiths, R. R., Richards, W. A., McCann, U., & Jesse, R. (2006). Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology, 187(3), 268–283.
Griffiths, R. R., Richards, W., Johnson, M., McCann, U., & Jesse, R. (2008). Mystical-type experiences occasioned by psilocybin mediate the attribution of personal meaning and spiritual significance 14 months later. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England), 22(6), 621–632.
Gröblacher, S., Paterek, T., Kaltenbaek, R., Brukner, Č., Zukowski, M., Aspelmeyer, M., & Zeilinger, A. (2007). An experimental test of non-local realism. Nature.
Grof, S. (2000). LSD-Psychotherapie. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.
Gruner, P., & Pittenger, C. (2017). Cognitive inflexibility in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Neuroscience, 345, 243–255.
Guglielmi, G. (2018). Doomsday Clock now only two minutes to Armageddon. Nature 2018.
Guiltinan, J. (2009). Creative destruction and destructive creations: Environmental ethics and planned obsolescence. Journal of Business Ethics.
Halberstadt, A. L. (2016). Behavioral and pharmacokinetic interactions between monoamine oxidase inhibitors and the hallucinogen 5-methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 143, 1–10.
Halpern, J. H., Lerner, A. G., & Passie, T. (2016). A Review of Hallucinogen Persisting Perception Disorder (HPPD) and an Exploratory Study of Subjects Claiming Symptoms of HPPD. In Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences (pp. 333–360).
Hameroff, S., & Penrose, R. (2014). Consciousness in the universe: A review of the Orch OR theory. Physics of Life Reviews, 11(1), 39–78.
Handsteiner, J., Friedman, A. S., Rauch, D., Gallicchio, J., Liu, B., Hosp, H., … Zeilinger, A. (2017). Cosmic Bell Test: Measurement Settings from Milky Way Stars. Physical Review Letters, 118(6).
Hariri, A. R. (2002). Serotonin Transporter Genetic Variation and the Response of the Human Amygdala. Science, 297(5580), 400–403.
Harman, W. W., McKim, R. H., Mogar, R. E., Fadiman, J., & Stolaroff, M. J. (1966). Psychedelic agents in creative problem-solving: a pilot study. Psychological Reports, 19(1), 211–227.
Hartmann, E., Rosen, R., & Rand, W. (1998). Personality and dreaming: Boundary structure and dream content. Dreaming.
Harvey, D. (2007). Neoliberalism as Creative Destruction. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 610(1), 21–44.
Heal, D. J., Smith, S. L., Gosden, J., & Nutt, D. J. (2013). Amphetamine, past and present – A pharmacological and clinical perspective. Journal of Psychopharmacology.
Heard, E., & Martienssen, R. A. (2014). Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: Myths and mechanisms. Cell.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. The Organization of Behavior, 911(1), 335.
Heinz, A., Braus, D. F., Smolka, M. N., Wrase, J., Puls, I., Hermann, D., … Büchel, C. (2005). Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic variation of the serotonin transporter. Nature Neuroscience.
Hermanns, W., & Einstein, A. (1983). Einstein and the Poet: In Search of the Cosmic Man.
Hesslow, G. (2012). The current status of the simulation theory of cognition. Brain Research, 1428, 71–79.
Hill, D., & Kumar, R. (2009). Global neoliberalism and education and its consequences. Routledge.
Hiriyanna, M. (1995). The essentials of Indian philosophy. Motilal Banarsidass Publishers.
Hoffman, D. D. (2016). The Interface Theory of Perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(3), 157–161.
Hofmann, A., Frey, A., Ott, H., Petrzilka, T., & Troxler, F. (1958). Elucidation of the structure and the synthesis of psilocybin. Experientia, 14(11), 397–399.
Hofmann, A., Heim, R., Brack, A., & Kobel, H. (1958). Psilocybin, ein psychotroper Wirkstoff aus dem mexikanischen Rauschpilz Psilocybe mexicana Heim. Experientia.
Hofmann, A., Heim, R., Brack, A., Kobel, H., Frey, H., Ott, H., … Troxler, F. (1959). Psilocybin und Psilocin, zwei psychotrope Wirkstoffe aus mexikanischen Rauschpilzen. Helvetica Chimica Acta, 42, 1557–1572.
Hofmann, A., & Troxler, F. (1959). Identifizierung von Psilocin. Experientia, 15(3), 101–102.
Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing Cultures: The Hofstede Model in Context. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1).
Holder, R. D. (2002). Fine‐Tuning, Multiple Universes and Theism. Noûs, 36(2), 295–312.
Horan, R. (2009). The Neuropsychological Connection Between Creativity and Meditation. Creativity Research Journal, 21(2–3), 199–222.
Hornung, J.-P. (2003). The human raphe nuclei and the serotonergic system. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy, 26(4), 331–343.
Hoshino, T., & Shimodaira, K. (1936). Über die Synthese des Bufotenin-Methyl-Äthers (5-Methoxy-N-Dimethyl-Tryptamin) und Bufotenins. Synthesen in der Indol-Gruppe. XV. Bulletin of the Chemical Society of Japan, 11(3), 221–224.
Hutchinson, D. A., & Savitzky, A. H. (2004). Vasculature of the parotoid glands of four species of toads (Bufonidae: Bufo). Journal of Morphology, 260(2), 247–254.
Huxley, A. (1954). The Doors of Perception and Heaven and Hell. Harper & Brothers, London.
Hwang, J., Kim, S., & Lee, D. (2009). Temporal discounting and inter-temporal choice in rhesus monkeys. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 9.
Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. A. (2015). Predicting Creativity: Interactive effects of openness to experience and emotion regulation ability. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(4), 480–487.
Jackson, F. (1986). What Mary Didn’t Know. The Journal of Philosophy, 83(5), 291–295.
Jacobsen, A. (2014). Operation Paperclip: The secret intelligence program to bring Nazi scientists to America. Little, Brown and Company.
Jacobsen, A. (2015). The Pentagon’s brain : an uncensored history of DARPA, America’s top secret military research agency. Little, Brown US.
Jacobsen, A. (2017). Phenomena: the Secret History of the U.S. Government’s Investigations into Extrasensory Perception and Psychokinesis. Little, Brown and Company.
Jacobus, J., & Tapert, S. F. (2013). Neurotoxic Effects of Alcohol in Adolescence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9(1), 703–721.
James, W. (1882). Subjective effects of nitrous oxide. Mind, 7.
James, W. (1890). The hidden self. Scribners, 361–373.
James, W. (1904). A World of Pure Experience. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1(20), 533.
James, W. (1985). The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study in Human Nature. London: Penguin Classics. (Originally published in 1902).
Jay, M. (2019). Mescaline: a global history of the first psychedelic. Yale University Press.
Jensen, H. (1932). Chemical Studies on Toad Poisons, IV, Bufagin and Cinobufagin. Science, 75(1932), 53–54.
Jimenez de Asúa, L. (1951). Nullum crimen, nulla poena sine lege. Zeitschrift Für Die Gesamte Strafrechtswissenschaft, 63(1).
Johansen, P.-Ø., & Krebs, T. S. (2015). Psychedelics not linked to mental health problems or suicidal behavior: A population study. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(3), 270–279.
Johnson, M., Richards, W., & Griffiths, R. (2008). Human hallucinogen research: guidelines for safety. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22(6), 603–620.
Jones, P. N. (2007). The Native American Church, Peyote, and Health: Expanding Consciousness for Healing Purposes. Contemporary Justice Review, 10(4), 411–425.
Josipovic, Z. (2010). Duality and nonduality in meditation research. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 1119–1121.
Josipovic, Z. (2014). Neural correlates of nondual awareness in meditation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1307(1), 9–18.
Jung, C. G. (1969). Aion: Researches into the Phenomenology of the Self, Collected Works of C. G. Jung (Volume 9). Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.
Kaczynski, T. J. (1995). Industrial society and its future. Jolly Roger Press.
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1991). Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status Quo Bias. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5(1), 193–206.
Kandel, R. E. (2015). The Age of Insight: The Quest to Understand the Unconscious in Art, Mind and Brain. The effects of brief mindfulness intervention on acute pain experience: An examination of individual difference (Vol. 1).
Kapoor, H., & Khan, A. (2017). Deceptively yours: Valence-based creativity and deception. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 23, 199–206.
Kapoor, P., Chugh, Y., Kapoor, A., & Sinha, U. S. (2008). Torture, terrorist and Truth Serum, 10, 64–70.
Kauder, E. (1899). Über Alkaloide aus Anhalonium Lewinii. Archiv Der Pharmazie, 237(3), 190–198.
Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Counting the muses: Development of the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS). Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 298–308.
Kaufman, S. B., Quilty, L. C., Grazioplene, R. G., Hirsh, J. B., Gray, J. R., Peterson, J. B., & Deyoung, C. G. (2016). Openness to Experience and Intellect Differentially Predict Creative Achievement in the Arts and Sciences. Journal of Personality, 84(2), 248–258.
Kavan, H. (2004). Glossolalia and altered states of consciousness in two New Zealand religious movements. Journal of Contemporary Religion, 19(2), 171–184.
Kaye, J. (2010). Obama Interrogation Official Linked to U.S. Mind Control Research. Retrieved from: pubrecord.org/torture/7730/obama-inter.
Keane, H. (2008). Pleasure and discipline in the uses of Ritalin. International Journal of Drug Policy, 19(5), 401–409.
Kekulé, A. (1866). Untersuchungen über aromatische Verbindungen. Annalen Der Chemie Und Pharmacie, 137(2), 129–196.
Kekulé, A. (1890). Benzolfest: Rede. Berichte Der Deutschen Chemischen Gesellschaft, 23(1), 1302–1311.
Keller, L. M. (2004). Is Truth Serum Torture? SSRN Electronic Journal.
Key, D., & Kerr, M. (2011). The Ouroboros (Part I): Towards an ontology of connectedness in ecopsychology research. European Journal of Ecopsychology.
Kim, Y., Teylan, M. A., Baron, M., Sands, A., Nairn, A. C., & Greengard, P. (2009). Methylphenidate-induced dendritic spine formation and DeltaFosB expression in nucleus accumbens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(8), 2915–2920.
King, L. A. (2013). Legal Classification of Novel Psychoactive Substances: An International Comparison. In Novel Psychoactive Substances: Classification, Pharmacology and Toxicology.
Kischka, U., Kammer, T. H., Maier, S., Weisbrod, M., Thimm, M., & Spitzer, M. (1996). Dopaminergic modulation of semantic network activation. Neuropsychologia, 34(11), 1107–1113.
Koçak, O. M., Kale, E., & Çiçek, M. (2012). Default Mode Network Connectivity Differences in Obsessive-compulsive Disorder. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 54(3–4), 118–124.
Konar, A. K. (2012). Defamiliarizing the Demographic Doomsday Diagram. Mother Pelican, 8(2).
Korotayev, A., & Malkov, A. (2016). A Compact Mathematical Model of the World System Economic and Demographic Growth, 1 CE – 1973 CE. International Journal of Mathematical Models and Methods in Applied Sciences, 10.
Kraehenmann, R., Preller, K. H., Scheidegger, M., Pokorny, T., Bosch, O. G., Seifritz, E., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2015). Psilocybin-induced decrease in amygdala reactivity correlates with enhanced positive mood in healthy volunteers. Biological Psychiatry, 78(8), 572–581.
Krebs-Thomson, K., Ruiz, E. M., Masten, V., Buell, M., & Geyer, M. A. (2006). The roles of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2 receptors in the effects of 5-MeO-DMT on locomotor activity and prepulse inhibition in rats. Psychopharmacology, 189(3), 319–329.
Krebs, T. S., & Johansen, P.-Ø. (2013). Psychedelics and Mental Health: A Population Study. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e63972.
Krishnamurti, J. (1969). Freedom from the known. Rider.
Kuhn, H. G., Biebl, M., Wilhelm, D., Li, M., Friedlander, R. M., & Winkler, J. (2005). Increased generation of granule cells in adult Bcl-2-overexpressing mice: A role for cell death during continued hippocampal neurogenesis. European Journal of Neuroscience.
Kuhn, T. (1970). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. University of Chicago Press, University of Chicago.
L’Etang, J. (1999). The father of spin: Edward L. Bernays and the birth of public relations. Public Relations Review, 25(1), 123–124.
Lachenmeier, D. W., & Rehm, J. (2015). Comparative risk assessment of alcohol, tobacco, cannabis and other illicit drugs using the margin of exposure approach. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 8126.
Lakoff, G. (1986). A Figure of Thought. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 1(3), 215–225.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1981). Metaphors We Live by. University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from : how the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. Basic Books.
Lamb, T. D. (2013). Evolution of phototransduction, vertebrate photoreceptors and retina. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 36, 52–119.
Langmead, C. J., & Christopoulos, A. (2013). Supra-physiological efficacy at GPCRs: superstition or super agonists? British Journal of Pharmacology, 169(2), 353–356.
Leary, M. R., Diebels, K. J., Davisson, E. K., Jongman-Sereno, K. P., Isherwood, J. C., Raimi, K. T., … Hoyle, R. H. (2017). Cognitive and Interpersonal Features of Intellectual Humility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
Lebedev, A. V., Kaelen, M., Lövdén, M., Nilsson, J., Feilding, A., Nutt, D. J., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2016). LSD-induced entropic brain activity predicts subsequent personality change. Human Brain Mapping, 37(9), 3203–3213.
Lee, C. S., Huggins, A. C., & Therriault, D. J. (2014). A measure of creativity or intelligence? Examining internal and external structure validity evidence of the Remote Associates Test. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(4), 446–460.
Lerner, M. J. (1997). What Does the Belief in a Just World Protect Us From: The Dread of Death or the Fear of Understanding Suffering? Psychological Inquiry, 8(1), 29–32.
Levine, J. (1983). Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly.
Lewin, L. (1894). Über Anhalonium Lewinii und andere Cacteen. Archiv Für Experimentelle Pathologie Und Pharmakologie.
Lewis, S. L., & Maslin, M. A. (2015). Defining the Anthropocene. Nature, 519(7542), 171–180.
Lima da Cruz, R. V., Moulin, T. C., Petiz, L. L., & Leão, R. N. (2018). A Single Dose of 5-MeO-DMT Stimulates Cell Proliferation, Neuronal Survivability, Morphological and Functional Changes in Adult Mice Ventral Dentate Gyrus. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 11, 312.
Lin, P., Bekey, G., & Abney, K. (2008). Autonomous Military Robotics: Risk, Ethics, and Design. California Polytechnic State University.
Lyons, T., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2018). Increased nature relatedness and decreased authoritarian political views after psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(7), 811–819.
MacLean, K. A., Johnson, M. W., & Griffiths, R. R. (2011). Mystical experiences occasioned by the hallucinogen psilocybin lead to increases in the personality domain of openness. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25(11), 1453–1461.
Majić, T., Schmidt, T. T., & Gallinat, J. (2015). Peak experiences and the afterglow phenomenon: When and how do therapeutic effects of hallucinogens depend on psychedelic experiences? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(3), 241–253.
Mano, H., & Fukada, Y. (2006). A Median Third Eye: Pineal Gland Retraces Evolution of Vertebrate Photoreceptive Organs. Photochemistry and Photobiology.
Markoff, J. (2005). What the Dormouse Said: How the Sixties Counterculture Shaped the Personal Computer Industry. New York: Viking.
Marques, J. (2012). Consciousness at Work: A Review of Some Important Values, Discussed from a Buddhist Perspective. Journal of Business Ethics, 105(1), 27–40.
Martin, D. A., & Nichols, C. D. (2018). The effects of hallucinogens on gene expression. In Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences (Vol. 36, pp. 137–158).
Martorana, F., Gaglio, D., Bianco, M. R., Aprea, F., Virtuoso, A., Bonanomi, M., … Colangelo, A. M. (2018). Differentiation by nerve growth factor (NGF) involves mechanisms of crosstalk between energy homeostasis and mitochondrial remodeling. Cell Death & Disease, 9(3), 391.
Maruani, A., Dumas, G., Beggiato, A., Traut, N., Peyre, H., Cohen-Freoua, A., … Delorme, R. (2019). Morning Plasma Melatonin Differences in Autism: Beyond the Impact of Pineal Gland Volume. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10.
Maslow, A. (1962). The Psychology of Science. Gateway Editions.
Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. 2nd ed. Toward a psychology of being. 2nd ed.
Mausfeld, R. (2017). Warum schweigen die Lämmer? Wie Elitendemokratie und Neoliberalismus unsere Gesellschaft und unsere Lebensgrundlagen zerstören. Westend Verlag.
Mausfeld, R. (2019). Angst und Macht: Herrschaftstechniken der Angsterzeugung in kapitalistischen Demokratien. Westend Verlag.
May, L. T., Leach, K., Sexton, P. M., & Christopoulos, A. (2007). Allosteric Modulation of G Protein–Coupled Receptors. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, 47(1), 1–51.
McClintick, J. N., Xuei, X., Tischfield, J. A., Goate, A., Foroud, T., Wetherill, L., … Edenberg, H. J. (2013). Stress–response pathways are altered in the hippocampus of chronic alcoholics. Alcohol, 47(7), 505–515.
McCoy, A. W. (2006). A question of torture : CIA interrogation, from the Cold War to the War on Terror. Metropolitan Books/Henry Holt and Co.
McCoy, A. W. (2007). Science in Dachau’s shadow: HEBB, Beecher, and the development of CIA psychological torture and modern medical ethics. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 43(4), 401–417.
McCrae, R. R. (1994). Openness to Experience: Expanding the boundaries of Factor V. European Journal of Personality.
McDougall, W. (1930). The hormic psychology. In Psychologies of 1930. (pp. 3–36). Worcester: Clark University Press.
McKenna, T. (1992). Food of the gods : the search for the original tree of knowledge : a radical history of plants, drugs and human evolution. Rider.
Melley, T. (2017). War on Terror. In American Literature in Transition, 2000–2010 (pp. 275–288). Cambridge University Press.
Metzner, R. (2015). Allies for awakening : guidelines for productive and safe experiences with entheogens. Berkeley, CA: Green Earth Foundation & Regent Press.
Millière, R. (2017). Looking for the Self: Phenomenology, Neurophysiology and Philosophical Significance of Drug-induced Ego Dissolution. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11.
Minkov, M., & Hofstede, G. (2012). Hofstede’s Fifth Dimension. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 3–14.
Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Raskoff Zeiss, A. (1972). Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(2), 204–218.
Misra, B., & Sudarshan, E. C. G. (1977). The Zeno’s paradox in quantum theory. Journal of Mathematical Physics, 18(4), 756–763.
Monterosso, J., Piray, P., & Luo, S. (2012). Neuroeconomics and the Study of Addiction. Biological Psychiatry, 72(2), 107–112.
Moon, J. S., Kuza, C. M., & Desai, M. S. (2018). William James, Nitrous Oxide, and the Anaesthetic Revelation. Journal of Anesthesia History, 4(1), 1–6.
Moore, D. W., Bhadelia, R. A., Billings, R. L., Fulwiler, C., Heilman, K. M., Rood, K. M. J., & Gansler, D. A. (2009). Hemispheric connectivity and the visual-spatial divergent-thinking component of creativity. Brain and Cognition, 70(3), 267–272.
Moore, L. D., & Elkavich, A. (2008). Who’s using and who’s doing time: Incarceration, the war on drugs, and public health. American Journal of Public Health.
Moreno, J. D. (2012). Mind wars : brain science and the military in the twenty-first century. Bellevue Literary Press.
Mueller, I. (2005). Mathematics and the Divine in Plato. In Mathematics and the Divine (pp. 99–121).
Müller, B. C. N., Gerasimova, A., & Ritter, S. M. (2016). Concentrative meditation influences creativity by increasing cognitive flexibility. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.
Müller, K., Püschel, K., & Iwersen-Bergmann, S. (2013). Suizid unter Psilocin-Einfluss. Archiv Fur Kriminologie, 231, 193–198.
Müller, U. J., Voges, J., Steiner, J., Galazky, I., Heinze, H. J., Möller, M., … Kuhn, J. (2013). Deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens for the treatment of addiction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
Mullis, K. (2000). Dancing naked in the mind field. New York: Vintage Books.
Mumford, L. (1967). The myth of the machine. Harcourt, Brace & World.
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-Regulation and Depletion of Limited Resources: Does Self-Control Resemble a Muscle? Psychological Bulletin.
Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Moran, R. J., Brookes, M. J., Williams, T. M., Errtizoe, D., … Nutt, D. J. (2013). Broadband Cortical Desynchronization Underlies the Human Psychedelic State. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(38), 15171–15183.
Nagai, F., Nonaka, R., & Satoh Hisashi Kamimura, K. (2007). The effects of non-medically used psychoactive drugs on monoamine neurotransmission in rat brain. European Journal of Pharmacology, 559(2–3), 132–137.
Nagel, T. (1974). What Is It Like to Be a Bat? The Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435.
Namazi, H. R. (2017). The Complexity Based Analysis of the Correlation between Spider’s Brain Signal and Web. ARC Journal of Neuroscience, 2, 44.
Nelson, T. (1975). Computer Lib/Dream Machines. South Bend: Nelson.
Nichols, D. E. (2004). Hallucinogens. Pharmacology and Therapeutics.
Nichols, D. E. (2018a). Dark Classics in Chemical Neuroscience: Lysergic Acid Diethylamide (LSD). ACS Chemical Neuroscience.
Nichols, D. E. (2018b). N,N -dimethyltryptamine and the pineal gland: Separating fact from myth. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(1), 30–36.
Nour, M. M., Evans, L., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2017). Psychedelics, Personality and Political Perspectives. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 49(3), 182–191.
Nour, M. M., Evans, L., Nutt, D., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2016). Ego-Dissolution and Psychedelics: Validation of the Ego-Dissolution Inventory (EDI). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 269.
Nunn, N., & Qian, N. (2010). The Columbian Exchange: A History of Disease, Food, and Ideas. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 24(2), 163–188.
O’Mara, S. (2009). Torturing the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(12), 497–500.
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47(6), 419–427.
Osmond, H. (1957). A review of the clinical effects of psychotomimetic agents. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 66(3), 418–434.
Ostafin, B. D., & Kassman, K. T. (2012). Stepping out of history: Mindfulness improves insight problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition.
Ott, J. (2001). Pharmepéna-psychonautics: Human intranasal, sublingual and oral pharmacology of 5-Methoxy-N, N-Dimethyl-Tryptamine. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs.
Otterman, M. (2017). American Torture. Pluto Press.
Pachter, I. J., Zacharias, D. E., & Ribeirol, O. (1959). Indole Alkaloids of Acer Saccharinum (The Silver Maple), Dictyoloma Incanescens, Piptadenia Colubrina, And Mimosa hostilis. Journal of Organic Chemistry.
Panossian, A., Wikman, G., Kaur, P., & Asea, A. (2012). Adaptogens Stimulate Neuropeptide Y and Hsp72 Expression and Release in Neuroglia Cells. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6.
Parkinson, T. D., Kornelsen, J., & Smith, S. D. (2019). Trait Mindfulness and Functional Connectivity in Cognitive and Attentional Resting State Networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
Passamonti, L., Terracciano, A., Riccelli, R., Donzuso, G., Cerasa, A., Vaccaro, M. G., … Quattrone, A. (2014). Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. NeuroImage.
Peet, M. (1979). Transcendental meditation. BMJ, 1(6157), 201–201.
Peres, A. (1980). Zeno paradox in quantum theory. American Journal of Physics, 48(11), 931–932.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 123–205).
Philipchalk, R., & Mueller, D. (2000). Glossolalia and temperature change in the right and left cerebral hemispheres. International Journal of Phytoremediation.
Phillips, J., Frances, A., Cerullo, M. A., Chardavoyne, J., Decker, H. S., First, M. B., … Zachar, P. (2012a). The six most essential questions in psychiatric diagnosis: A pluralogue. Part 4: General conclusion. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine.
Phillips, J., Frances, A., Cerullo, M. A., Chardavoyne, J., Decker, H. S., First, M. B., … Zachar, P. (2012b). The six most essential questions in psychiatric diagnosis: A pluralogue part 1: Conceptual and definitional issues in psychiatric diagnosis. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine.
Phillips, J., Frances, A., Cerullo, M. A., Chardavoyne, J., Decker, H. S., First, M. B., … Zachar, P. (2012c). The six most essential questions in psychiatric diagnosis: A pluralogue part 2: Issues of conservatism and pragmatism in psychiatric diagnosis. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine.
Phillips, J., Frances, A., Cerullo, M. A., Chardavoyne, J., Decker, H. S., First, M. B., … Zachar, P. (2012d). The six most essential questions in psychiatric diagnosis: A pluralogue part 3: Issues of utility and alternative approaches in psychiatric diagnosis. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine.
Power, R. A., Steinberg, S., Bjornsdottir, G., Rietveld, C. A., Abdellaoui, A., Nivard, M. M., … Stefansson, K. (2015). Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder predict creativity. Nature Neuroscience, 18(7), 953–955.
Prabhu, V., Sutton, C., & Sauser, W. (2008). Creativity and certain personality traits: Understanding the mediating effect of intrinsic motivation. Creativity Research Journal.
Price, D. H. (2007). Buying a piece of anthropology Part 1: Human Ecology and unwitting anthropological research for the CIA. Anthropology Today, 23(3), 8–13.
Provencio, I., Rodriguez, I. R., Jiang, G., Hayes, W. P., Moreira, E. F., & Rollag, M. D. (2000). A Novel Human Opsin in the Inner Retina. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20(2), 600–605.
Quine, W. V, & Ullian, J. S. (1978). The Web of Belief. Social Sciences/Languages.
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hirstein, W. (1999). The Science of Art A Neurological Theory of Aesthetic Experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies.
Rätsch, C. (1998). Enzyklopädie der psychoaktiven Pflanzen : Botanik, Ethnopharmakologie und Anwendung. Wissenschaftliche Verlagsgesellschaft.
Rauter, T. (2017). Nullum Crimen Sine Lege. In Judicial Practice, Customary International Criminal Law and Nullum Crimen Sine Lege (pp. 19–86). Cham: Springer International Publishing.
Régnier, C., Achaz, G., Lambert, A., Cowie, R. H., Bouchet, P., & Fontaine, B. (2015). Mass extinction in poorly known taxa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(25), 7761–7766.
Riga, M. S., Soria, G., Tudela, R., Artigas, F., & Celada, P. (2014). The natural hallucinogen 5-MeO-DMT, component of Ayahuasca, disrupts cortical function in rats: reversal by antipsychotic drugs. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 17(08), 1269–1282.
Ripple, W. J., Wolf, C., Newsome, T. M., Galetti, M., Alamgir, M., Crist, E., … Laurance, W. F. (2017). World Scientists’ Warning to Humanity: A Second Notice. BioScience, 67(12), 1026–1028.
Rizzo, P., Tinello, C., Pearlstein, R. A., & Taniuchi, H. (1999). Antibody immunodiversity: A study on the marked specificity difference between two anti-yeast iso-1 cytochrome c monoclonal antibodies whose epitopes are closely related. Journal of Protein Chemistry.
Roberts, T. B. (2006). Psychedelic horizons : Snow White, immune system, multistate mind, enlarging education. Imprint Academic.
Rocke, A. J. (2015). It began with a daydream: the 150th anniversary of the Kekulé benzene structure. Angewandte Chemie (International Ed. in English), 54(1), 46–50.
Rose, N., & Abi-Rached, J. (2014). Governing through the Brain: Neuropolitics, Neuroscience and Subjectivity. The Cambridge Journal of Anthropology, 32(1).
Roseman, L., Demetriou, L., Wall, M. B., Nutt, D. J., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2018). Increased amygdala responses to emotional faces after psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Neuropharmacology, 142, 263–269.
Roseman, L., Sereno, M. I., Leech, R., Kaelen, M., Orban, C., McGonigle, J., … Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2016). LSD alters eyes-closed functional connectivity within the early visual cortex in a retinotopic fashion. Human Brain Mapping, 37(8), 3031–3040.
Rossi, C., Angelucci, A., Costantin, L., Braschi, C., Mazzantini, M., Babbini, F., … Caleo, M. (2006). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is required for the enhancement of hippocampal neurogenesis following environmental enrichment. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24(7), 1850–1856.
Roth, B. L., Baner, K., Westkaemper, R., Siebert, D., Rice, K. C., Steinberg, S., … Rothman, R. B. (2002). Salvinorin A: A potent naturally occurring nonnitrogenous opioid selective agonist. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(18), 11934–11939.
Routledge, C. D., & Arndt, J. (2009). Creative terror management: Creativity as a facilitator of cultural exploration after mortality salience. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
Ruck, C. A. P., Bigwood, J., Staples, D., Ott, J., & Wasson, G. (1979). Entheogens. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 11(1–2), 145–146.
Rucker, J. J. H. (2015). Psychedelic drugs should be legally reclassified so that researchers can investigate their therapeutic potential. BMJ, 350(may26 20), h2902–h2902.
Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., & Aaker, J. (2012). Awe Expands People’s Perception of Time, Alters Decision Making, and Enhances Well-Being. Psychological Science.
Russell, B. (1952). The impact of science on society. Routledge.
Russell, B. (1981). Mysticism and logic, and other essays. Barnes & Noble Books.
Sanchez-Ramos, J. (2015). The entourage effect of the phytocannabinoids. Annals of Neurology.
Sand, S. (2007). The Development of Psychological Torture: A Modern History of Coercive Interrogation and its Effectiveness.
Sapir, E. (1929). The Status of Linguistics as a Science. Language, 5(4), 207.
Saunders, R. (2012). Towards Autonomous Creative Systems: A Computational Approach. Cognitive Computation, 4(3), 216–225.
Saville, B. K., Gisbert, A., Kopp, J., & Telesco, C. (2010). Internet Addiction and Delay Discounting in College Students. The Psychological Record, 60(2), 273–286.
Schartner, M. M., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Barrett, A. B., Seth, A. K., & Muthukumaraswamy, S. D. (2017). Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine, LSD and psilocybin. Scientific Reports, 7(April), 46421.
Schläpfer, T., & Kayser, S. (2014). Tiefe Hirnstimulation bei therapieresistenter Depression. Klinische Neurophysiologie, 45(02), 113–117.
Schlosshauer, M., Kofler, J., & Zeilinger, A. (2013). A snapshot of foundational attitudes toward quantum mechanics. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part B – Studies in History and Philosophy of Modern Physics, 44(3), 222–230.
Schoch, R. (1994). A Conversation with Kerry Mullis. California Monthly, 105(1), 20.
Schredl, M., Atanasova, D., Hörmann, K., Maurer, J. T., Hummel, T., & Stuck, B. A. (2009). Information processing during sleep: The effect of olfactory stimuli on dream content and dream emotions. Journal of Sleep Research.
Schwartz, T. W., & Holst, B. (2007). Allosteric enhancers, allosteric agonists and ago-allosteric modulators: where do they bind and how do they act? Trends in Pharmacological Sciences.
Scott, W. A. (2006). Cognitive Complexity and Cognitive Flexibility. Sociometry.
Sessa, B. (2012). Shaping the renaissance of psychedelic research. The Lancet, 380(9838), 200–201.
Sheldrake, R., McKenna, T. K., Abraham, R., & Abraham, R. (2001). Chaos, creativity, and cosmic consciousness. Park Street Press.
Shen, H.-W., Jiang, X.-L., Winter, J. C., & Yu, A.-M. (2010). Psychedelic 5-methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine: metabolism, pharmacokinetics, drug interactions, and pharmacological actions. Current Drug Metabolism, 11(8), 659–666.
Shen, H. W., Wu, C., Jiang, X. L., & Yu, A. M. (2010). Effects of monoamine oxidase inhibitor and cytochrome P450 2D6 status on 5-methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine metabolism and pharmacokinetics. Biochemical Pharmacology.
Shen, W., Liu, C., Zhang, X., Zhao, X., Zhang, J., Yuan, Y., & Chen, Y. (2013). Right Hemispheric Dominance of Creative Insight: An Event-Related Potential Study. Creativity Research Journal, 25(1), 48–58.
Shen, X., Finn, E. S., Scheinost, D., Rosenberg, M. D., Chun, M. M., Papademetris, X., & Constable, R. T. (2017). Using connectome-based predictive modeling to predict individual behavior from brain connectivity. Nature Protocols, 12(3), 506–518.
Shulgin, A. A., & Shulgin, A. A. (1997). TIHKAL. The continuation. Transform Press.
Shulgin, A. T., & Carter, M. F. (1980). N,N-Diisopropyltryptamine (DIPT) and 5-Methoxy-N,N-diisoppropyltryptamine (5-MeO-DIPT). Two orally active tryptamine analogs with CNS Activity. Communications in Psychopharmacology.
Shultes, E. R. (1976). A Golden Guide: Hallucinogenic Plants. New York.
Siegel, D. J. (2010). Mindsight : the new science of personal transformation. California: Bantam Books.
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’Connor, A. (2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: Exploring lower-order, high-order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 1087–1090.
Singer, P. (1990). Ethics and animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(1), 45–48.
Sivananda, S. R. I. S. (1972). Bhagavad gita. World, 23–24.
Slattery, D. (2015). Xenolinguistics : psychedelics, language, and the evolution of consciousness. Berkeley, California: North Atlantic Books.
Smigielski, L., Scheidegger, M., Kometer, M., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2019). Psilocybin-assisted mindfulness training modulates self-consciousness and brain default mode network connectivity with lasting effects. NeuroImage, 196, 207–215.
Smith, G., & Chomsky, N. (1987). On Power and Ideology: The Managua Lectures. Foreign Affairs, 65(5), 1105.
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the Relative Independence of Thinking Biases and Cognitive Ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Stanton, S. J., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Huettel, S. A. (2017). Neuromarketing: Ethical Implications of its Use and Potential Misuse. Journal of Business Ethics, 144(4), 799–811.
Stapp, H. (2001). Quantum theory and the role of mind in nature. Foundations of Physics, 31(10), 1465–1499.
Stapp, H. (2007). Quantum Mechanical Theories of Consciousness. In The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness (pp. 300–312).
Steffen, W., Broadgate, W., Deutsch, L., Gaffney, O., & Ludwig, C. (2015). The trajectory of the Anthropocene: The Great Acceleration. The Anthropocene Review, 2(1), 81–98.
Steffen, W., Rockström, J., Richardson, K., Lenton, T. M., Folke, C., Liverman, D., … Schellnhuber, H. J. (2018). Trajectories of the Earth System in the Anthropocene. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(33), 8252–8259.
Strassman, R. (2001). DMT: the spirit molecule. Rochester, VT, (March), 7821.
Sugarman, J. (2015). Neoliberalism and psychological ethics. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 35(2), 103–116.
Sun, J., Chen, Q., Zhang, Q., Li, Y., Li, H., Wei, D., … Qiu, J. (2016). Training your brain to be more creative: brain functional and structural changes induced by divergent thinking training. Human Brain Mapping, 37(10), 3375–3387.
Sunstein, C. R., & Vermeule, A. (2008). Conspiracy Theories. SSRN Electronic Journal.
Sunstein, C. R., & Vermeule, A. (2009). Conspiracy Theories: Causes and Cures*. Journal of Political Philosophy, 17(2), 202–227.
Szabo, A. (2015). Psychedelics and immunomodulation: Novel approaches and therapeutic opportunities. Frontiers in Immunology, 6(JUN).
Tagliazucchi, E., Roseman, L., Kaelen, M., Orban, C., Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Murphy, K., … Carhart-Harris, R. (2016). Increased Global Functional Connectivity Correlates with LSD-Induced Ego Dissolution. Current Biology, 26(8), 1043–1050.
Tanner, R. G. (1970). Δianoia and plato’s cave. The Classical Quarterly, 20(1), 81–91.
Tart, C. T. (1972). States of Consciousness and State-Specific Sciences. Science (New York, N.Y.), 176(4040), 1203–1210.
Tart, C. T. (2008). Altered states of consciousness and the spiritual traditions: The proposal for the creation of state-specific sciences. In Handbook of Indian psychology. (pp. 577–609).
Taylor, T., Raine, K., & Harper, G. M. (1969). Thomas Taylor, the Platonist: selected writings. Princeton University Press.
Teh, L. K., & Bertilsson, L. (2012). Pharmacogenomics of CYP2D6: Molecular Genetics, Interethnic Differences and Clinical Importance. Drug Metabolism and Pharmacokinetics, 27(1), 55–67.
Tetlock, P. E., Mellers, B. A., Rohrbaugh, N., & Chen, E. (2014). Forecasting Tournaments. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(4), 290–295.
Tomasi, D., & Volkow, N. D. (2010). Functional connectivity density mapping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(21), 9885–9890.
Torres, C. M., & Repke, D. B. (2006). Anadenanthera : visionary plant of ancient South America. Haworth Herbal Press.
Torres, C. M., Repke, D. B., Chan, K., Mckenna, D., Llagostera, A., & Schultes, R. E. (1991). Snuff Powders from Pre-Hispanic San Pedro de Atacama: Chemical and Contextual Analysis. Current Anthropology, 32(5), 640–649.
Tosini, G. (1997). The pineal complex of reptiles: physiological and behavioral roles. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 9(4), 313–333.
Trerotola, M., Relli, V., Simeone, P., & Alberti, S. (2015). Epigenetic inheritance and the missing heritability. Human Genomics.
Troxler, F., Seemann, F., & Hofmann, A. (1959). Abwandlungsprodukte von Psilocybin und Psilocin. 2. Mitteilung Über synthetische Indolverbindungen. Helvetica Chimica Acta.
Tupper, K. W. (2002). Entheogens and Existential Intelligence: The Use of Plant Teachers as Cognitive Tools. Canadian Journal of Education / Revue Canadienne de l’éducation, 27(4), 499.
Twarog, B. M. (1988). Serotonin: History of a discovery. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part C: Comparative Pharmacology, 91(1), 21–24.
Uthaug, M. V., Lancelotta, R., van Oorsouw, K., Kuypers, K. P. C., Mason, N., Rak, J., … Ramaekers, J. G. (2019). A single inhalation of vapor from dried toad secretion containing 5-methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT) in a naturalistic setting is related to sustained enhancement of satisfaction with life, mindfulness-related capacities, and a decrement of psyc. Psychopharmacology.
Vaadia, E. (2009). Grand challenges of brain computer interfaces in the years to come. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3(2), 151–154.
van den Bos, W., & McClure, S. M. (2013). Towards a general model of temporal discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99(1), 58–73.
Vartanian, O. (2013). Fostering Creativity: Insights from Neuroscience. In J. Vartanian, O., Bristol, A. & Kaufman (Ed.), Neuroscience of Creativity (pp. 257–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Venn, J. (1880). On the diagrammatic and mechanical representation of propositions and reasonings. Philosophical Magazine Series 5, 59(10), 1–18.
Vitali, S., Glattfelder, J. B., & Battiston, S. (2011). The network of Global corporate control. PLoS ONE.
von Förster, H. (2003). Understanding understanding: essays on cybernetics and cognition. Springer.
von Förster, H., Mora, P. M., & Amiot, L. W. (1960). Doomsday: Friday, 13 November, A.D. 2026. Science, 132(3436), 1291–1295.
Von Neumann, J. (1955). Mathematical Foundations of Quantum Mechanics. American Mathematical Monthly (Vol. 72).
Walczyk, J. J., & Griffith-Ross, D. A. (2008). Commentary on the Functional Creativity Model: Its Application to Understanding Innovative Deception. Creativity Research Journal, 20(2), 130–133.
Walczyk, J. J., Runco, M. A., Tripp, S. M., & Smith, C. E. (2008). The Creativity of Lying: Divergent Thinking and Ideational Correlates of the Resolution of Social Dilemmas. Creativity Research Journal, 20(3), 328–342.
Wallach, J. V. (2009). Endogenous hallucinogens as ligands of the trace amine receptors: A possible role in sensory perception. Medical Hypotheses, 72(1), 91–94.
Wals, A. E. J., & Jickling, B. (2002). “Sustainability” in higher education: from doublethink and newspeak to critical thinking and meaningful learning. Higher Education Policy, 15(2), 121–131.
Walsh, C. (2016). Psychedelics and cognitive liberty: Reimagining drug policy through the prism of human rights. International Journal of Drug Policy, 29, 80–87.
Wang, C., Brunton, E., Haghgooie, S., Cassells, K., Lowery, A., & Rajan, R. (2013). Characteristics of electrode impedance and stimulation efficacy of a chronic cortical implant using novel annulus electrodes in rat motor cortex. Journal of Neural Engineering.
Wasson, G. R., Hofmann, A., & Ruck, C. A. P. (1979). The Road to Eleusis: Unveiling the Secret of the Mysteries. The Classical World, 73, 1–28.
Watson, D. (2003). To dream, perchance to remember: Individual differences in dream recall. Personality and Individual Differences.
Weinberger, S. (2011). Spies to use Twitter as crystal ball. Nature.
Weitemier, A. Z., & Ryabinin, A. E. (2003). Alcohol-induced memory impairment in trace fear conditioning: A hippocampus-specific effect. Hippocampus, 13(3), 305–315.
Williams, A. D., & Grisham, J. R. (2012). Impulsivity, emotion regulation, and mindful attentional focus in compulsive buying. Cognitive Therapy and Research.
Winkelman, M. (2004). Shamanism as the original neurotheology. Zygon, 39(1), 193–217.
Winkelman, M. (2015). Psychedelics as Medicines for Substance Abuse Rehabilitation: Evaluating Treatments with LSD, Peyote, Ibogaine and Ayahuasca. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 7(2), 101–116.
Winter, A. (2011). Manchurian Candidates: Forensic Hypnosis in the Cold War. Grey Room, 45, 106–127.
Winter, J. ., Filipink, R. ., Timineri, D., Helsley, S. ., & Rabin, R. . (2000). The Paradox of 5-Methoxy-N,N-Dimethyltryptamine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 65(1), 75–82.
Wiseman, H. (2015). Quantum physics: Death by experiment for local realism. Nature, 526(7575), 649–650.
Witt, P. N. (1951). d-Lysergsäure-diäthylamid (LSD 25) im Spinnentest. Experientia, 7(8), 310–311.
Wolkowitz, O. M., Mellon, S. H., Epel, E. S., Lin, J., Dhabhar, F. S., Su, Y., … Blackburn, E. H. (2011). Leukocyte telomere length in major depression: Correlations with chronicity, inflammation and oxidative stress – preliminary findings. PLoS ONE, 6(3), e17837.
Wolman, B. B. (1981). Hormic and Holistic Theories. In Contemporary Theories and Systems in Psychology (pp. 180–196). Boston, MA: Springer US.
Worm, B., Barbier, E. B., Beaumont, N., Duffy, J. E., Folke, C., Halpern, B. S., … Watson, R. (2006). Impacts of biodiversity loss on ocean ecosystem services. Science, 314(5800), 787–790.
Wright, I. A., & Ortiz, F. (1941). Contrapunteo Cubano del Tabaco y el Azucar. The Hispanic American Historical Review, 21(3), 459.
Wu, J. (1999). Serotonin and learned helplessness: a regional study of 5-HT1A, 5-HT2A receptors and the serotonin transport site in rat brain. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 33(1), 17–22.
Young, G. (2008). Capgras delusion: An interactionist model. Consciousness and Cognition.
Yu, K., Liu, C., Yu, T., Wang, X., Xu, C., Ni, D., & Li, Y. (2018). Out-of-body experience in the anterior insular cortex during the intracranial electrodes stimulation in an epileptic child. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 54, 122–125.
Zakharov, A. V., Khivintseva, E. V., Pyatin, V. F., Sergeeva, M. S., & Antipov, O. I. (2019). Melatonin – Known and Novel Areas of Clinical Application. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology.
Zeese, B. K., & Flowers, M. (2014). Lifting the Veil of Mirage Democracy in the United States. Truthout, (February 2013), 1–13.
Zhang, J., Rane, G., Dai, X., Shanmugam, M. K., Arfuso, F., Samy, R. P., … Sethi, G. (2016). Ageing and the telomere connection: An intimate relationship with inflammation. Ageing Research Reviews, 25, 55–69.
Zhuk, O., Jasicka-Misiak, I., Poliwoda, A., Kazakova, A., Godovan, V., Halama, M., & Wieczorek, P. (2015). Research on acute toxicity and the behavioral effects of methanolic extract from psilocybin mushrooms and psilocin in mice. Toxins, 7(4), 1018–1029.

Top